Reasoning and Decision-Making
Read: Anderson: Chapters 10 11
PSYC 575
Research Paper: Final Submission Assignment Instructions
Overview
You will write a research paper that discusses and evaluates the current research in the field of false memories. As a formal research paper, it must be completely focused on the empirical evidence pertaining to the topic. Refrain from discussing your personal opinions or experiences. You must use scholarly sources for your references; do not use a textbook, website, or popular press as a source.
Instructions
The paper must include the following:
· 1214 pages of content (not including title page, abstract, and reference page)
· Title, abstract and reference page
· Current APA formatting throughout
· At least 12 peer-reviewed journal articles
· A discussion of the important limitations of the evidence (studies) presented
· A discussion of conflicting evidence for any of the studies discussed
The organization of this paper must be as follows:
Title Page 1 page
Abstract 120150 words
Introduction 12 paragraphs
Thesis statement: one sentence that states the focus of your paper
1. Key Points (include 35 main points)
a. Point 1
b. Point 2
c. Point 3
d. Point 4
e. Point 5
Body of Paper 810 pages
Topic sentence and supporting research for each of your key points. Use the same order as in the introduction.
1. Research Concept/Finding
a. Supporting evidence
b. Connect to next concept/finding
2. Research Concept/Finding
a. Supporting evidence
b. Connect to next concept/finding
3. Research Concept/Finding
a. Supporting evidence
b. Connect to next concept/finding
Summary 12 pages
Write a summary sentence that wraps up the concepts discussed in the paper.
1. Summary sentence must be followed by clear statements that summarize each of the main concepts/findings discussed in the body.
a. Summary of research Concept/Finding 1
b. Summary of research Concept/Finding 2
c. Summary of research Concept/Finding 3
d. Summary of research Concept/Finding 4
e. Summary of research Concept/Finding 5
Conclusion 1 paragraph
Final thoughts in a paragraph
References At least 12 peer-reviewed journal articles
Note: Your assignment will be checked for originality via the Turnitin plagiarism tool.
Page 1 of 2
Cognitive Psychology
and Its Implications
Eighth Edition
ANDERSON8e-FM.indd 1 13/09/14 10:04 AM
This page intentionally left blank
John R. Anderson
Carnegie Mellon University
w o r t h P u b l i s h e r s
A Macmillan Education Company
Cognitive Psychology
and Its Implications
Eighth Edition
ANDERSON8e-FM.indd 3 13/09/14 10:04 AM
u To Gordon Bower
Vice President, Editing, Design, and Media Production: Catherine Woods
Publisher: Rachel Losh
Associate Publisher: Jessica Bayne
Senior Acquisitions Editor: Christine Cardone
Marketing Manager: Lindsay Johnson
Marketing Assistant: Tess Sanders
Development Editor: Len Neufeld
Associate Media Editor: Anthony Casciano
Assistant Editor: Catherine Michaelsen
Director of Editing, Design, and Media Production: Tracey Kuehn
Managing Editor: Lisa Kinne
Project Editor: Kerry OShaughnessy
Art Director: Diana Blume
Cover Designer: Vicki Tomaselli
Text Designer: Dreamit Inc.
Illustration Coordinator: Janice Donnola
Illustrations: Dragonfly Media Group
Photo Editor: Bianca Moscatelli
Production Manager: Sarah Segal
Composition: MPS Ltd.
Printing and Binding: RR Donnelley and Sons
Cover Painting: Mario Colonel/Aurora/Getty Images
Library of Congress Control Number: 2014938514
ISBN-13: 978-1-4641-4891-0
ISBN-10: 1-4641-4891-0
© 2015, 2010, 2005, 2000 by Worth Publishers
All rights reserved.
Printed in the United States of America
First Printing
Worth Publishers
41 Madison Avenue
New York, NY 10010
www.worthpublishers.com
ANDERSON8e-FM.indd 4 13/09/14 10:04 AM
http://www.worthpublishers.com
John Robert Anderson is Richard King Mellon Professor of Psychology
and Computer Science at Carnegie Mellon University. He is known for developing
ACT-R, which is the most widely used cognitive architecture in cognitive science.
Anderson was also an early leader in research on intelligent tutoring systems,
and computer systems based on his cognitive tutors currently teach mathematics
to about 500,000 children in American schools. He has served as President of
the Cognitive Science Society, and has been elected to the American Academy
of Arts and Sciences, the National Academy of Sciences, and the American
Philosophical Society. He has received numerous scientific awards including the
American Psychological Associations Distinguished Scientific Career Award, the
David E. Rumelhart Prize for Contributions to the Formal Analysis of Human
Cognition, and the inaugural Dr. A. H. Heineken Prize for Cognitive Science. He
is completing his term as editor of the prestigious Psychological Review.
v
About the A
E-mail address: [email protected] (S. Fusi).
Neurocomputing 38}40 (2001) 1223}1228
Long term memory:
Encoding and storing strategies of the brain
Stefano Fusi
Institute of Physiology, University of Bern, Bu( hlplatz 5, 3012 Bern, Switzerland
Abstract
Plastic material devices, either arti”cial or biological, should be capable of rapidly modifying
their internal state to acquire information and, at the same time, preserve it for long periods (the
stability}plasticity dilemma). Here we compare, in a simple and intuitive way, memory stability
against noise of two di!erent strategies based, respectively, on fully analog devices that
accumulate linearly small changes and on systems with a limited number of stable states and
threshold mechanisms. We show that the discrete systems are more stable, even with short
inherent time constants, and can easily exploit the noise in the input to control the learning rate.
We “nally demonstrate the strategy by discussing a model of a biologically plausible spike-
driven synapse. ? 2001 Elsevier Science B.V. All rights reserved.
Keywords: Synaptic plasticity; Long term memory; Learning
1. Introduction
Material (arti”cial or biological) learning devices, like the synapses, have the
capability of changing their internal states in order to acquire (learn) and store
(memorize) information about the statistics of the incoming #ux of stimulations. In
a realistic situation, the stimulations carrying relevant information are separated
by long time intervals of noisy input which tends to erase the memory of the
previously acquired information.Moreover the interference of novel stimulations with
already acquired older ;memories’ may give rise to memory loss (e.g. the oldest
stimulations are forgotten to make room for the new ones). This is also known as the
0925-2312/01/$ – see front matter ? 2001 Elsevier Science B.V. All rights reserved.
PII: S 0 9 2 5 – 2 3 1 2 ( 0 1 ) 0 0 5 7 1 – 9
stability}plasticity dilemma: the memory should be stable against irrelevant inputs
(e.g. noise) for long periods and, at the same time, the internal state should be rapidly
modi”ed to acquire the information conveyed by the relevant inputs. This dilemma
becomes particularly arduous when dealing with material memory devices that do not
allow arbitrarily large time constants or parameters “ne tuning, especially if the
devices are small (e.g. it is reasonable to assume that permanent changes can not be
arbitrarily small).
Here we show one possible encoding and storing strategy that solves this dilemma
and we exemplify it by discussing a model of a spike-driven learning synapse. The
strategy is based on the assumption that information to be coded is redundant: e.g. for
the s
J Sleep Res. 2022;31:e13527. wileyonlinelibrary.com/journal/jsr | 1 of 12
https://doi.org/10.1111/jsr.13527
1 | INTRODUC TION
Previous studies report that false memories can be influenced by
sleep (for a review see Conte ; Ficca, 2013; Landmann et al., 2014).
Among the first to investigate the relationship between sleep and
false memories, Diekelmann, Born, and Wagner (2010), Diekelmann,
Landolt, Lahl, Born, and Wagner (2008) showed that sleep- deprived
individuals produce more false memories at morning re- test com-
pared to participants in an undisturbed sleep condition. The au-
thors specified that this effect could be mainly linked to an impaired
memory retrieval process. In fact, acute sleep loss can affect several
cognitive functions related to prefrontal activity that are essential
to accurate recall from long- term memory (Durmer ; Dinges, 2005;
Frenda ; Fenn, 2016).
Received: 28 June 2021 | Revised: 16 November 2021 | Accepted: 18 November 2021
DOI: 10.1111/jsr.13527
R E S E A R C H A R T I C L E
False memories formation is increased in individuals with
insomnia
Serena Malloggi1 | Francesca Conte2 | Oreste De Rosa2 | Stefania Righi1 |
Giorgio Gronchi1 | Gianluca Ficca2 | Fiorenza Giganti1
1Department of NEUROFARBA,
University of Florence, Florence, Italy
2Department of Psychology, University of
Campania L. Vanvitelli, Caserta, Italy
Correspondence
Serena Malloggi, Department of
NEUROFARBA, University of Florence,
Via di San Salvi 12, 50135, Florence, Italy.
Email: [email protected]
Funding information
V:ALERE 2019
Summary
Previous studies suggest that sleep can influence false memories formation.
Specifically, acute sleep loss has been shown to promote false memories production
by impairing memory retrieval at subsequent testing. Surprisingly, the relationship
between sleep and false memories has only been investigated in healthy subjects
but not in individuals with insomnia, whose sleep is objectively impaired compared
to healthy subjects. Indeed, this population shows several cognitive impairments
involving prefrontal functioning that could affect source monitoring processes and
contribute to false memories generation. Moreover, it has been previously reported
that subjects with insomnia differentially process sleep- related versus neutral stimuli.
Therefore, the aim of the present study was to compare false memories production
between individuals with insomnia symptoms and good sleepers, and to evaluate
the possible influence of stimulus category (neutral versus sleep- related) in the two
groups. The results show that false memories are globally increased in participants re-
porting insomnia symptoms compared to
Cognitive reserve modulates ERPs associated with verbal
working memory in healthy younger and older adults
Megan E. Speer1 and Anja Soldan2
Megan E. Speer: [email protected]; Anja Soldan: [email protected]
1Department of Psychology, Rutgers University, Newark, NJ 07102
2Department of Neurology, Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine, Baltimore, MD, 21205
Abstract
Although many epidemiological studies suggest the beneficial effects of higher cognitive reserve
(CR) in reducing age-related cognitive decline and dementia risk, the neural basis of CR is poorly
understood. To our knowledge, the current study represents the first electrophysiological
investigation of the relationship between CR and neural reserve (i.e., neural efficiency and
capacity). Specifically, we examined whether CR modulates event-related potentials (ERPs)
associated with performance on a verbal recognition memory task with three set sizes (1, 4, or 7
letters) in healthy younger and older adults. Neural data showed that as task difficulty increased,
the amplitude of the parietal P3b component during the probe phase decreased and its latency
increased. Notably, the degree of these neural changes was negatively correlated with CR in both
age groups, such that individuals with higher CR showed smaller changes in P3b amplitude and
less slowing in P3b latency (i.e., smaller changes in the speed of neural processing) with
increasing task difficulty, suggesting greater neural efficiency. These CR-related differences in
neural efficiency may underlie reserve against neuropathology and age-related burden.
Keywords
Cognitive reserve; Verbal working memory; Neural efficiency; Event-related potentials (ERPs);
P3; Cognitive aging
© 2015 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
Corresponding Author: Dr. Anja Soldan, Ph.D., Division of Cognitive Neuroscience, 1620 McElderry Street, Reed Hall, Baltimore,
MD 21205, Telephone: 410-502-2188, FAX: 410-502-2189, [email protected]
Megan E. Speer, Department of Psychology, Rutgers University, Newark; Anja Soldan, Cognitive Neuroscience Division, Department
of Neurology, Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine.
Publisher’s Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our
customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting,
Priming analogical reasoning with false memories
Mark L. Howe ; Sarah R. Garner ; Emma Threadgold ;
Linden J. Ball
Published online: 18 March 2015
# The Author(s) 2015. This article is published with open access at Springerlink.com
Abstract Like true memories, false memories are capable of
priming answers to insight-based problems. Recent research
has attempted to extend this paradigm to more advanced
problem-solving tasks, including those involving verbal ana-
logical reasoning. However, these experiments are
constrained inasmuch as problem solutions could be generat-
ed via spreading activation mechanisms (much like false
memories themselves) rather than using complex reasoning
processes. In three experiments we examined false memory
priming of complex analogical reasoning tasks in the absence
of simple semantic associations. In Experiment 1, we demon-
strated the robustness of false memory priming in analogical
reasoning when backward associative strength among the
problem terms was eliminated. In Experiments 2a and 2b,
we extended these findings by demonstrating priming on new-
ly created homonym analogies that can only be solved by
inhibiting semantic associations within the analogy. Overall,
the findings of the present experiments provide evidence that
the efficacy of false memory priming extends to complex an-
alogical reasoning problems.
Keywords Priming . Analogical reasoning . False memory .
DRMparadigm . Homonyms . Spreading activation
Memory is highly flexible and reconstructive, designed to
retain information about the past, interpret the present, and
support simulations of future events (e.g., Howe, 2011;
Newman ; Lindsay, 2009; Schacter, Guerin, ; St. Jacques,
2011). Interestingly, recent research has shown that memory is
highly functional, regardless of whether we are talking about
memories for events that actually occurred (i.e., true memo-
ries) or for self-generated memories of events that did not
occur (i.e., false memories). For example, a significant body
of research has demonstrated that true memories are able to
prime performance on related memory tasks (e.g., implicit
memory; see Gulan ; Valerjev, 2010) as well as non-
memory tasks such as verbal problem solving (e.g.,
Mednick, Mednick, ; Mednick, 1964).
Priming refers to Ba change in the ability to identify, pro-
duce, or classify an item as a result of a prior encounter with
that item, or a related item^ (Schacter, Gallo, ; Kensinger,
2007, p. 356). In the case of analogical reasoning, for exam-
ple, there is a well-established body of evidence demonstrat-
ing that people are able to transfer directly their prior memo-
ries of problems and their solutions in order to assist them in
tackling new, related problems (e.g., Bassok ; Holyoak,
1989; Richland, Zur, ; Holyoak, 2007; for a recent review,
see Holyoak, 2012). Alt
https://doi.org/10.1177/2167702618797106
Clinical Psychological Science
2019, Vol. 7(1) 29 31
© The Author(s) 2018
Article reuse guidelines:
sagepub.com/journals-permissions
DOI: 10.1177/2167702618797106
www.psychologicalscience.org/CPS
ASSOCIATION FOR
PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCECommentary
Patihis and Pendergrasts (2019; this issue, p. 3) research
raises questions about balancing risks of false memories
with risks of not treating childhood trauma that may
have been forgotten. Their central concern is that when
clinicians ask about repressed memory, many clients
will form false memories of child abuse.
They emphasize suggestibility/false memory, but
their review omits important studies that moderate their
concerns. For example, when Pezdek, Finger, and
Hodge (1997) tried to implant a false memory in adults
of receiving a childhood enema, the error rate was zero.
Although some adults, including with trauma histories,
agree with schema-consistent false suggestions about
childhood events, when it comes to taboo acts of a
sexual nature, Goldfarb, Goodman, Larson, Eisen, and
Qin (in press) again found zero false memories. Many
suggestibility/false memory studies use creative coding,
such as when partial false memories (That never
happened to me, but if my mother said it did, it could
have been at the mall) are nearly buried in statistics
reported.
That said, we acknowledge the reality of false abuse
memories in some individuals as possibly induced or
encouraged by therapists, particularly those who use
hypnosis or psychotropic drugs (e.g., in combination
with religious or other doctrines; Bottoms, Shaver, ;
Goodman, 1996). Still, it is unclear that clinicians should
refrain from discussion with clients about lost memory
(a term we prefer because it does not invoke repres-
sion mechanistically), given that therapy can help
memory: Child victims who sought therapy during/soon
after legal involvement (vs. did not) had more accurate
long-term memory for abuse a decade later (Goodman,
Goldfarb, Quas, ; Lyon, 2017).
We note that Patihis and Pendergrast (2019) leave
largely unaddressed that lost memories of childhood
trauma can occur, as can recovery of them. In longitu-
dinal studies of documented child sexual abuse (CSA),
15% to 38% of victims failed to recall the target case. Of
those who recalled it, 15% to 16% said there were times
of not remembering it (Goodman et al., 2003; Williams,
1994). More severe abuse was associated with reporting
having forgotten the CSA but also actually predicted
greater memory accuracy for it (Ghetti et al., 2006).
Relevant are our past findings regarding misinterpreta-
tion of research questions. Victims often misinterpreted
questions about repressed memory and amnesia,
wrongly assuming we were asking if there was a time
when they were not consciously thinking ab
Sleep deprivation increases formation of false memory
JUNE C . LO , PEARLYNNE L . H . CHONG , SHANKAR I GANESAN ,
RUTH L . F . L EONG and M I CHAEL W . L . CHEE
Centre for Cognitive Neuroscience, Neuroscience and Behavioral Disorders Program, Duke-NUS Medical School, Singapore
Keywords
adolescents, adults, cognitive function, false
memory, memory formation, sleep deprivation
Correspondence
Michael W. L. Chee, Centre for Cognitive
Neuroscience, Duke-NUS Graduate Medical
School, 169857 Singapore.
Tel.: +65 65164916;
fax: +65 62218625;
e-mail: [email protected]
[The copyright line for this article was changed
on 17 February 2017 after original online
publication.]
Accepted in revised form 22 May 2016; received
25 September 2015
DOI: 10.1111/jsr.12436
SUMMARY
Retrieving false information can have serious consequences. Sleep is
important for memory, but voluntary sleep curtailment is becoming more
rampant. Here, the misinformation paradigm was used to investigate
false memory formation after 1 night of total sleep deprivation in healthy
young adults (N = 58, mean age ? SD = 22.10 ? 1.60 years; 29
males), and 7 nights of partial sleep deprivation (5 h sleep opportunity)
in these young adults and healthy adolescents (N = 54, mean
age ? SD = 16.67 ? 1.03 years; 25 males). In both age groups,
sleep-deprived individuals were more likely than well-rested persons to
incorporate misleading post-event information into their responses during
memory retrieval (P < 0.050). These findings reiterate the importance of
adequate sleep in optimal cognitive functioning, reveal the vulnerability of
adolescents memory during sleep curtailment, and suggest the need to
assess eyewitnesses sleep history after encountering misleading
information.
INTRODUCTION
Memories of an event rarely provide a literal record of that
experience. Instead, they involve the integration of elements
of that episode with prior experience or knowledge. A highly
novel or distinct experience, for example a first publication in
a high-impact journal, is rarely mis-remembered. However,
when the memory of a specific episode is confused with prior
similar experiences, and/or fails to be distinctly encoded,
errors in subsequent memory retrieval can occur. The
emergence of such false memories often reminds us of
human fallibility, as highlighted by the inconsistencies in
recollection of personal events surrounding the Challenger
disaster (Neisser and Harsch, 1992). However, they can also
have more serious consequences such as wrongful convic-
tion due to inaccurate eyewitness testimony.
Adequate sleep is essential to optimize memory processes
(Diekelmann and Born, 2010; Stickgold and Walker, 2013).
This is consistent across a range of tests eval
COMP R E H EN S I V E R E V I EW
Induction of false beliefs and false memories in laboratory
studiesA systematic review
Beate Muschalla | Fabian Schönborn
Institute of Psychology, Technische Universität
Braunschweig, Braunschweig, Germany
Correspondence
Prof. Dr. Beate Muschalla, Technische
Universität Braunschweig, Institute of
Psychology, Humboldtstraße 33, 38106
Braunschweig, Germany.
Email: [email protected]
Abstract
Psychological interventions often use guided discovery and other techniques for
diagnostic exploration and intervention planning. This way, memories may arise in
the person, which may be true or false. False memories of earlier events can be harm-
ful and result in real suffering, similar to actual traumatic memories. Based on cogni-
tive psychological and psycho-traumatological findings, there is pronounced dissent
in the academic disciplines regarding the conceptualization, relevance and research
of false memories. This review contributes to the basic question of how often false
beliefs and false memories may be induced within the frame of different interactional
techniques. A systematic review has been conducted of 59 articles from (quasi-)
experimental studies and two qualitative sources from 30 data bases. Three main
methods of memory induction provide the basis for reporting: imagination inflation,
false feedback, and memory implantation. Due to the conceptual and methodological
diversity of the studies, the results appear to be heterogeneous. Free and guided
imagery, as well as suggestive statements, could induce false beliefs or false memo-
ries in, on average, 20%50% of the participants who underwent experimental
manipulation concerning false past events. A false belief induction may occur after
dream interpretation or hypnosis in more than 50% of participants. Personalized sug-
gestion is more effective in inducing memory than the general plausibility of the
suggested events. Further research questions are which therapeutic actions seem
appropriate in cases of harmful false memories. This depends not only on whether
there are veridical elements in the false memory but also on the quality and meaning
of the memory for the person’s life and ability to cope with burdens.
K E YWORD S
false beliefs, false memories, guided imagery, memory induction, side effects, suggestion
1 | INTRODUCTION
False belief is present when an individual is erroneously convinced that
an event has happened to or with them earlier in their life, even if
they pr