Chat with us, powered by LiveChat PLEASE DO NOT SUBMIT A BID FOR THIS IF YOU DO NOT HAVE EXPERIENCE WITH GRADUATE-LEVEL WRITING TERMS - STUDENT SOLUTION USA

PLEASE DO NOT SUBMIT A BID FOR THIS IF YOU DO NOT HAVE EXPERIENCE WITH GRADUATE-LEVEL WRITING TERMS AND CONCEPTS. MUST FOLLOW ALL INSTRUCTIONS MUST BE FOLLOWED AND NO PLAGIARISM.  USE THE SCHOLARLY SOURCES INCLUDES.  

Week 2 – Assignment

Diversity and Ethical Codes

A manager at your company overhears another employee who is Caucasian directing the N-word at an African American employee. When she confronts him, he claims that he was using it as a term of endearment—a claim that is not explicitly contested by the “friend” to whom he’s directing it. The manager, concerned that her being Caucasian and in a position of authority, fears her intervention may escalate things, so she chose to accept the explanation and move on. The manager has come to you, the Human Resources Manager, for advice.

In a 1,050- to 1,400-word (or 3- to 4-page) paper (excluding references and title page), discuss the following:

Your company’s diversity code states:

“As team members, we have a responsibility to:

· Do our part to help Acme to serve and earn business from a wide variety of communities and stakeholders.

· Integrate diversity into our sourcing processes.

· Help create an environment in which all team members can contribute, develop, and fully use their talents.

· Keep an open mind to new ideas and listen to different points of view.”

1. What are some of the limitations of the company’s diversity code for multicultural professional practice?

2. In what ways do you believe this code is culturally biased and culturally encapsulated?

3. What evidence is there that this code is culturally sensitive?

4. If you believe there is no evidence that the code is culturally sensitive, what evidence is there that the code is not culturally sensitive?

5. Discuss the importance of cultural sensitivity and explain what you believe are the implications for ethical professional practice.

6. In addition to the required readings, cite at least two additional references that include examples of a better ethical code.  

Resources

Required References

Blanding, M. (2013, December 9). How Cultural Conflict Undermines Workplace Creativity [Web page]. Retrieved from 

https://www.forbes.com/sites/hbsworkingknowledge/2013/12/09/how-cultural-conflict-undermines-workplace-creativity/#3cce89db214f (Links to an external site.)

Christie, P., Kwon, I., Stoeberl, P., & Baumhart, R. (2003, September). A cross-cultural comparison of ethical attitudes of business managers: India, Korea and the United States. Journal of Business Ethics, 46(3), 263-287.

Weber, Z. (2004). Working towards culturally sensitive ethical practice in a multicultural society. Journal of Practice Teaching 5(3), 40-54. Retrieved from 

http://journals.whitingbirch.net/index.php/JPTS/article/download/314/346

Murugavel, V., & Somaraju, A. (2016). CULTURAL DIFFERENCES ON CONFLICT STRATEGIES IN THE WORKPLACE. International Journal of Arts & Sciences, 9(2), 135-144. https://www.proquest.com/scholarly-journals/cultural-differences-on-conflict-strategies/docview/1858849736/se-2?accountid=32521

Das Neves, J. C., & Melé, D. (2013). Managing Ethically Cultural Diversity: Learning from Thomas Aquinas: JBE. Journal of Business Ethics, 116(4), 769-780. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10551-013-1820-1

Zita Weber

40 Journal of Practice Teaching 5(3) 2004, pp.40-54 © 2004. Whiting and Birch

Working towards culturally
sensitive ethical practice in a
multicultural society

Zita Weber1

Summary: In the last two decades there has been a lot of theorising about mul-
ticulturalism and professional practice. Many practitioners are challenged by
cultural diversity daily. Practising from a culturally sensitive ethical perspective
in a multicultural society is essential for good practice. Postmodern infl uences
and critical questioning are seen to inform culturally sensitive ethical practice
in their encouragement of practitioners’ adoption of multiple belief systems
and multiple perspectives and the need to pose questions challenging practice
regarding awareness of cultural encapsulation and cultural sensitivity. The
development of culturally sensitive ethical practice guidelines within the context
of a multicultural society is proposed, fi rstly, by assessing the cultural sensitivity
of the ethics codes and secondly, by balancing culture and ethical codes.

Key words: professional practice, cultural competence, ethics, cultural diversity,
multiple perspectives, respect

1. Lecturer in Social Work Practice

Address for Correspondence: School of Social Work and Policy Studies,
The University of Sydney, Education Building, Sydney, NSW 2006, Australia.
[email protected]

brought to you by COREView metadata, citation and similar papers at core.ac.uk

provided by Whiting & Birch (E-Journals)

Working towards culturally sensitive ethical practice in a multicultural society

41 Journal of Practice Teaching 5(3) 2004, pp.40-54 © 2004. Whiting and Birch

In the last two decades, there has been a lot written about multicultural-
ism and professional practice (Diaz-Lazaro and Cohen, 2001; Dominelli,
1988; Ivey, 1987; Ivey, Ivey and Simek-Morgan, 1997; Johnson, 1989;
Ivey, Pedersen and Ivey, 2001; Nguyen and Bowles, 1998; Pedersen,
2000; Sue and Sue, 1977; Sue and Sue, 2003; Thompson, 1997;
Wohl, 1989). Many of us are challenged by cultural diversity daily.
Of late, some literature has explored the heightened awareness of the
importance of cultural considerations in practice and the realisation that
culturally competent practice and ethical decision-making need close
refl ection (Goldberg, 2000; Ivey, Pedersen and Ivey, 2001; Pack-Brown,
Whittington-Clark and Parker, 1998; Pack-Brown and Williams, 2000;
Pack-Brown and Williams, 2003).

My own thinking has become sharper over time. Because I’d been a
migrant child, I always believed I had some real understanding and a
deep level of empathy for cultural differences and sensibilities. However,
over the years, I’ve discovered how much I had to learn about the realities
of working with cultural diversity.

Teaching social work students at both undergraduate and graduate
levels in Australia, a country recognised as comprising many different
languages, cultural traditions and ethnic origins, I’m confronted by
cultural sensitivity and competence issues and the ever-present ethical
issues and dilemmas.

Post modern infl uences

Working towards culturally sensitive ethical practice presupposes a
post modern stance (Christopher, 1996; Sue et al., 1996), and a move
away from epistemological assumptions that are not necessarily shared
across cultures. Such a stance entertains the existence of multiple belief
systems and multiple perspectives (Gonzalez, 1997; Highlen, 1996; Sue
et al, 1996). For instance, culturally sensitive practice would accept
the existence of multiple worldviews and reinforce the notion that
such worldviews are neither ‘good or bad’ nor ‘right or wrong’. The
refutation of this ‘either or’ view endorses the validity of each worldview
and reinforces cultural relativism by recognising that each culture and
attendant worldview is unique and can only be understood in itself
and not by reference to any other culture and attendant worldview.

Zita Weber

42 Journal of Practice Teaching 5(3) 2004, pp.40-54 © 2004. Whiting and Birch

Socio-politically, this stance is important as it inherently recognises the
unfairness of one group imposing its standards on another.

Postmodernism also would inform the practitioner working towards
culturally sensitive ethical practice that language does not equal
‘perception of reality’. Adopting a relational view of language allows
the practitioner to look beyond the truths and realities of the dominant
culture as enshrined in language, both oral and written. Language and
conceptual constructions vary tremendously between cultures. The
importance of sensitivity to language and the way in which it is used
may be illustrated in the encounter where the practitioner is using
language and formulating questions from their cultural perspective
regarding mental health concerns and the client is viewing the concerns
from their different worldview. Such a situation is exemplifi ed by Tsui
and Schultz (1985) who write,

The therapist must explicitly educate the client about the purpose of
questions regarding clinical history, previous treatment information,
family background and psychosocial stressors. The linkage of these issues
to their current symptoms is not clear to many Asian clients. Many Asian
clients conceive of mental distress as the result of physiological disorder
or character fl aws. This issue must be dealt with sensitively before any
sensible therapeutic work can be effected. (pp.567-568)

Similarly, it is important to take into consideration cultural nuances
of nonverbal cues as different cultural groups ascribe varied meaning
to certain nonverbal behaviour. Eye contact, for instance, is expected
among persons communicating in mainstream English-speaking cultures
(Australia, Britain, USA, Canada). Certain stereotypes have developed
regarding evasiveness and untrustworthiness of those people who avoid
direct gazing. However, it is now well known that in some cultures,
direct eye contact is regarded as disrespectful and an invasion of privacy.
I experienced this cultural nuance when teaching in Texas where some
Mexican-American students employed minimal eye contact with me.
When I enquired about this, it was explained that as a older person in
position of authority, it would be disrespectful to look me directly in
the eye.

Failing to understand the signifi cance of nonverbal behaviour may
pose barriers to effective communication. Again, I experienced a cultural
nuance in expectation when working with an Asian family. Whilst they

Working towards culturally sensitive ethical practice in a multicultural society

43 Journal of Practice Teaching 5(3) 2004, pp.40-54 © 2004. Whiting and Birch

expected me to provide advice as the ‘authority’ in the matter, I expected
them to be more talkative and active in exploring options in the care of
a member of the family who had been diagnosed with a major mental
illness. Respect for authority may result in passivity and silence and as
Tsui and Schultz (1985) note, ‘Long gaps of silence may occur as the
client waits patiently for the therapist to structure the interview, take
charge and thus provide the solution’ (p.565). Erroneously concluding
that the client has fl at affect or is unmotivated are potential hazards if
the practitioner fails to correctly interpret the often smallest cultural
nuance in nonverbal cues.

Although a post modern stance provides philosophical underpinnings
that embrace the phenomena of cultural diversity and cultural sensitivity,
translating concepts into dynamic, evolving practice poses some critical
questions. Differences and nuances create challenges to our balancing
of professional obligation with expectation of service delivery and
determinations regarding the distinction between client behaviour that
is culturally appropriate and behaviour that is problematic.

Some critical questions

How can we ensure that we develop and maintain cultural sensitivity?
What of the danger of cultural encapsulation? Can we develop culturally
sensitive ethical guidelines and bring culturally appropriate interpreta-
tions to our work? How do we go about teaching about values and ethics
in a multicultural context? These are some questions that both students
and practitioners ask at teaching and learning forums.

In one sense, the only way to address such questions is to pose
further questions that challenge our ways of thinking. To avoid cultural
encapsulation and a practice that is infused with Western assumptions
and values we need to use all our critical refl ective abilities to deconstruct
the sometimes narrowly prescribed ways of working. We need to pose
questions that challenge known practice around (i) not giving advice
and suggestions because it may foster dependency (ii) not taking a
teaching role (iii) not accepting gifts from clients and (iii) not entering
into dual or multiple relationships because establishing boundaries is
important (Sue et al, 1998).

What if the encounter with a client from a different culture demands

Zita Weber

44 Journal of Practice Teaching 5(3) 2004, pp.40-54 © 2004. Whiting and Birch

that you exhibit the expertise and authority that otherwise would be
discouraged as qualities that magnify power differentials between you
and your client? What if the client’s view about mental illness, for
instance, is based on beliefs regarding magic and witchcraft, however,
they ask that you educate them on terminology and the biomedical point
of view, so that they might understand what is being communicated to
them? What if the client proffers a small gift and presses you to accept
at your last session together? What if …. There are endless ‘what if…’
questions we might pose to challenge our ways of thinking and ensure
greater cultural sensitivity.

Essentially, these crucial questions point to the logic of returning to
the fundamentals.

Whenever I pick up the Australian Association of Social Work (AASW)
Code of Ethics (2000), I’m struck by two competing thoughts:

1. it is an essential resource – a valuable document;
2. it is a prescriptive document that has little interpretive value and if

followed to the letter, one would be unable to practice.

There is the ambivalence of feeling contented that I have a reference
book and the frustration that I’m told so little when it comes to sticky
situations.

This Code, along with the British, U.S. and Canadian codes, is neces-
sarily a broad-based document which can offer guidance, but cannot
and should not be relied upon to solve ethical dilemmas involving
cultural issues.

These codes contain concepts and ideas that are well-known among
helping professionals. Nevertheless, when considering culturally
sensitive and ethical practice, one crucial question arises: How can
abstractions within codes be interpreted? Subsequent key questions
could be: What questions should we be critically refl ecting upon to
become aware of the power of cultural variables? Most importantly,
how can we translate this awareness into behaviour leading to effective
intervention?

I intend to work towards developing culturally sensitive ethical
practice guidelines within the context of a multicultural society by
fi rstly, assessing the cultural sensitivity of the ethics codes and secondly,
balancing culture and ethical codes.

Working towards culturally sensitive ethical practice in a multicultural society

45 Journal of Practice Teaching 5(3) 2004, pp.40-54 © 2004. Whiting and Birch

Assessing cultural sensitivity of the ethics code

Professionals are expected to know and adhere to their ethics code.
However, practitioners also need to demonstrate knowledge about their
codes’ sensitivity to diverse cultures. While codes have embedded within
them requirements for cultural competence, for instance, in the Australian
Association of Social Workers’ Code of Ethics (2000) Section 4.2.4 is titled
Cultural Awareness – how do some other principles fi t with this?

For example, how do some cultural assumptions and expectations
of our clients fi t with ethics codes’ statements regarding ‘professional
integrity’?

In the AASW Code of Ethics, Section 4.1.4 titled ‘Professional
Integrity’ under (e) states: ‘Social workers will ensure that professional
relationships are not exploited to gain personal, material or fi nancial
advantage’. This sounds perfectly reasonable. But how does the practice
of gift-giving to show appreciation to a practitioner fi t with the principle
of professional integrity as stated? There have been numerous occasions
in my practice as a social worker when ethnic families have brought gifts
to the fi nal session. Does acceptance of such gifts, offered in the spirit of
genuine appreciation, constitute some exploitative material gain? One
supervisor had cautioned me about never accepting any gifts, taking an
unqualifi ed position. Another equally experienced supervisor had taken
a much more relational position. Accepting the offered gift, in his view,
turned on the ‘it depends’ argument. Cultural considerations might well
be a dependent variable. Does accepting the gift suggest greater cultural
sensitivity but less ethical practice or does rejecting the gift signify less
sensitivity and greater adherence to ethical practice? Does a simplistic
position of following the code to the letter make for good practice
– culturally or otherwise? Perhaps the supervisor who recommended
a ‘it depends’ position was suggesting a more balanced and measured
approach, whereby cultural sensitivity was balanced with the spirit of,
rather than the bald words, within a code of ethics.

Section 4.1.4 ‘Professional Integrity’ includes under (g) cautions about
dual relationships. Similar provisions appear in the BASW Code p. 7
under 3.4 ‘Integrity’ 3.4.2 e ‘To set and enforce explicit and appropriate
professional boundaries to minimize the risk of confl ict, exploitation or
harm in all relationships with current or former service users, research
participants, students, supervisees or colleagues.’

Zita Weber

46 Journal of Practice Teaching 5(3) 2004, pp.40-54 © 2004. Whiting and Birch

Both these codes of ethics add a qualifi cation to this statement which, in
effect, softens the statement. This qualifi cation relates to minimising the ‘risk
of confl ict, exploitation or harm’. In the BASW Code p. 7 under 3.4.2.f states
‘To avoid any behaviour which may violate professional boundaries, result in
unintentional harm or damage the professional relationship.’

This qualifi cation is important and in order to understand what
professional integrity might look like in relation to cultural sensitivity,
it is worthwhile considering a relatively familiar scenario for many
practitioners.

Let’s take as an illustration the example of an invitation extended
to you by a client to attend a social event. This invitation may come
unexpectedly from a client or family you have been seeing for some time.
The client may be from an ethnic background which has a collectivistic
culture and she thinks of you, as her social worker, as ‘family’. She asks
that you attend her granddaughter’s christening celebration. She would
be ‘honoured’ if you did so.

This invitation poses a dilemma for you as you consider a response
that, on the one hand, conforms to ethical sanctions against dual rela-
tionships and, on the other, respects the client’s genuine valuing of you
within the context of her culture. In previous sessions, she has explained
that her cultural context embraces multiplicity in relationship roles and
that she has had her priest and her doctor to dinner. For this client, the
notion of strict professional boundaries is not part of her culture.

The questions that might crowd your mind could be:

• Is there a real risk of harm or exploitation if I attend the social
event?

• Do I risk harming the client/s if I attend?
• What decision, attending or not attending, would best respect the

client’s dignity?
• How can I respond in a way that refl ects the client’s worth as an

individual ?
(questions adapted from Pack-Brown and Williams, 2003)

Also crowding your mind might be ethical decision-making
frameworks to assist in working through dilemmas. One such model,
forwarded by Welfel (1998) proposes an ethical decision-making model
that consists of nine steps:

Working towards culturally sensitive ethical practice in a multicultural society

47 Journal of Practice Teaching 5(3) 2004, pp.40-54 © 2004. Whiting and Birch

1. develop ethical sensitivity
2. defi ne dilemma and options
3. refer to professional standards
4. search out ethics scholarship
5. apply ethical principles to the situation
6. consult with supervisor or respected colleagues
7. deliberate and decide
8. inform supervisor, implement and document actions
9. refl ect on the experience

Such a model encourages the sort of critical questioning and refl ection
that offers the comfort of systemic analysis yet pushes the practitioner
to consider the dilemma from several different perspectives (codes,
scholarship and colleagues’ views). Searching out ethics scholarship in
this instance, would necessitate attention to diversity and difference and
presuppose cultural sensitivity.

This framework implies balancing ethical considerations and for me,
effective practice must have a balance.

Balancing culture and ethical codes

In this case of the invitation to a social event, an infant’s christening,
several responses are possible, not all of which are sensitive to both the
client’s culture and ethical principles. What are some of the options?
What are their strengths and drawbacks?

Response 1

Explain to the client that your professional ethics code does not permit
your participation in her social event. This is an example of a response
emanating from a procedural perspective without consideration of
the client’s cultural context and practices. It would be fair to say that
this response is not a culturally sensitive one. In fact, the client might
be within her rights to beg the question, Who is protected here? The
practitioner? The client? Both?

Response 2

Engage the client in discussion around the importance to her for you to

Zita Weber

48 Journal of Practice Teaching 5(3) 2004, pp.40-54 © 2004. Whiting and Birch

attend. It might be argued that this response is more culturally sensitive
than the fi rst, however, it inherently questions the client’s motives,
suggesting to her that her request needs examination.

Response 3

Discuss with the client how she would feel if you did or did not attend.
This response is similar to Response 2, suggesting that her request
must be analysed. In addition, exploring her reactions to your options
is premature and likely to confuse the client.

Response 4

Immediately accept the client’s invitation to attend because you
believe not doing so would offend the client and risk the professional
relationship, which has been a long and positive one. This response,
of immediate acceptance, might suggest that the practitioner has not
carefully weighed the costs and benefi ts of agreeing to attend.

Response 5

Explain your dilemma to the client and tell her you wish to consult with
colleagues, including someone from her ethnic background before you
make a decision. In doing so, the practitioner positions the dilemma
as one related to ethical constraints rather than to the client’s request.
This response considers the process of weighing cultural factors against
ethical constraints. Nevertheless, by suggesting a consultation with
an ‘ethnic expert’, the practitioner inherently questions the client’s
knowledge and understanding of what is appropriate.

Response 6

Share with the client that you feel honoured to be asked to such an
important event in her life, but that her invitation presents a dilemma
for you. This might be seen as a culturally sensitive statement because
it positions the problem squarely in the practitioner’s hands and shares
the dilemma with the client, without demonstrating disrespect for the
client’s wisdom. This response might be followed by Response 3.

In my practice, I have known professionals from many varied back-
grounds to take very different positions on invitations to social events,
particularly when the request has come from a client from a different cul-
tural background. I have never seen any of them take the decision lightly

Working towards culturally sensitive ethical practice in a multicultural society

49 Journal of Practice Teaching 5(3) 2004, pp.40-54 © 2004. Whiting and Birch

and certainly, when I decided to not attend my client’s granddaughter’s
christening, I deliberated carefully, sought my supervisor’s opinion and
spoke with colleagues before deciding that although there was very
little risk of real harm to the client in my attending the event, doing
so would extend boundaries beyond their professional perimeters. My
discussion with the client acknowledged the importance culturally for
her inclusion of me in such an event and my respect for her cultural
position. However, our discussion also covered respect for different
positions and the need for us to continue in roles that clearly indicated
that our relationship was professional rather than friendly. Nevertheless,
I have known other practitioners who have attended social events and
argued that their need to be culturally sensitive was greater than their
need to maintain clear professional role identity.

There are many other examples of the need to balance culture and
ethical codes. Notions related to client welfare is one.

In the AASW code, Section 3.1 Value: Human Dignity and Worth
states that the social work profession holds that ‘each person has a right
to well-being, self-fulfi lment and self-determination, consistent with
the rights of others’. The NASW (U.S.) contains an ethical principle
valuing the ‘inherent dignity and worth of the person.’ Consistent with
this value, social workers are required to ‘treat each person in a caring
and respectful fashion, (being) mindful of individual differences and
cultural and ethnic diversity.’ There is a similar provision in the BASW
3.1.2b p.4

Certainly, the overarching principle contained in these sections of
various codes is protection of the welfare, or best interest, of the client.
Nevertheless, these statements are broad and they leave room for
interpretation and possibly, misinterpretation.

These codes, based as they are on principles of self-determination,
individualism and clear relational boundaries, may well advocate a
stance which is at odds with more interdependent, self-in-relation pat-
terns of some ethnic and cultural ways of being. These contradictions
may place practitioners in a bind – doing what is in the best interest
of the client may confl ict with various ethical codes. For instance, an
Aboriginal woman with terminal cervical cancer rejected all forms of
Western treatment in a large Sydney hospital and although not consid-
ered to be in her ‘best interests’ she insisted on discharging herself and
going ‘home’ to her country community to be cared for by her extended
family. For this Aboriginal woman, her self-determination could be

Zita Weber

50 Journal of Practice Teaching 5(3) 2004, pp.40-54 © 2004. Whiting and Birch

seen to run counter to her best interests. However, she felt alienated in
a big city hospital away from her family and her wish of ‘going home’
to be ‘with family’ was granted. In this case, the social worker and the
team balanced ethical codes with culture and believed that the client
would fare better in what she considered her own caring environment.
The abstraction of ‘best interests’ needs careful consideration in such a
case and self-determination regarding continuation or not of treatment
viewed from different perspectives. Client welfare is not an obvious
matter. Paradoxically, what appears to not be in the ‘best interests’ of
the client, might in reality, be her ‘best interests’.

Consider another situation. Clients from collectivist cultures for
whom self-identity is inseparable from kinship systems may wish to
bring family members with them to group therapy sessions. This may
pose a problem for the professional in terms of the importance of
maintaining the confi dentiality of group members, yet the professional
might understand that it is within the ‘best interests’ of the client to
have the support they want. In this case, the professional must balance
the rights of others and confi dentiality with the expectation of different
cultural groups and aim to negotiate a culturally sensitive decision. This
notion of collectivity and support from family is raised by Pedersen
(2000) who considers the concept of dependency as a potential source
of confl ict between ethical codes and culture.

In many diverse cultures, dependency on others is a way of life. Over
time, cultural expectations regarding strong networks of interdependence
have developed. Sometimes, socio-economic reasons have dictated that
such interdependence is critical to the survival of many immigrants as
well as indigenous people. Again a point of tension arises in that the
ethics codes advise against relationships in which clients feel dependent
on the professional. Perhaps several questions need to be asked to
tease out the elements of the dilemma. Is it possible that a dependent
relationship might be in the client’s ‘best interest’ at least temporarily?
Is it conceivable that a strong relationship with a professional might
provide a sense of security and help the client, who feels they have little
personal power, to build greater self-suffi ciency? Again, paradoxically,
the culturally sensitive practitioner might well be seen to be ‘encourag-
ing’ dependency in order to help the client become more empowered
eventually. Such tensions between individualism and collectivism and
empowerment and dependency are common in work with people
from diverse cultures and the ethical dilemmas they raise need careful

Working towards culturally sensitive ethical practice in a multicultural society

51 Journal of Practice Teaching 5(3) 2004, pp.40-54 © 2004. Whiting and Birch

refl ection and logical consideration. A mere wish by the professional
to be ‘politically correct’ is not enough, indeed, it runs counter to true
professionalism, which requires that the professional be in a position to
state clearly their dilemma and the reasons for their decision.

A cautionary note

There may be occasions when responding in a culturally sensitive
manner may mean taking an historical view and working with its
implications for clients in the contemporary context. For instance,
an important consequence of oppression of indigenous and minority
groups is the development of an intergenerational healthy cultural para-
noia phenomenon (Ho, 1992; Paniagua, 1994; Smith, 1981). Amongst
Australian Aboriginal people, for example, there is strong suspicion of
professionals in offi cial roles, particularly in relation to child welfare.
Historically, welfare authorities routinely removed children from parents,
a practice that has led to what is now known as ‘the lost generations’.
It makes perfect sense then, that Aboriginal people today might be
distrustful of professionals who offer help, but are also in a position to
recommend removal of children from families. Similarly, in the United
States, this healthy paranoia phenomenon is evident amongst some
African-American people who fi nd it diffi cult to trust professionals who
have disempowered them in the past (Smith, 1981).

However, the question might have to be asked: How do I determine
the difference between client behaviour that is culturally appropriate
and behaviour that is problematic? For instance, when does the healthy
cultural paranoia exhibited by some oppressed cultural groups become
problematic for them? Making that determination from a culturally
sensitive stance is the challenge.

Zita Weber

52 Journal of Practice Teaching 5(3) 2004, pp.40-54 © 2004. Whiting and Birch

Some concluding thoughts

Neither cultural encapsulation where the professional is trapped in one
way of thinking and believing that theirs is the universal way, nor a keen
sense of political correctness makes for culturally sensitive practice. The
professional who wishes to be culturally sensitive and competent and have
confi dence in making culturally sensitive ethical decisions, needs to live
with uncertainty and acknowledge the power of the postmodernist stance
regarding the existence, and legitimacy of, multiple worldviews. At all times,
such a practitioner is performing a delicate balancing act. They must be
guided by and bring together into a coherent whole, ethical decision-making
frameworks, what the ethics codes state, what the client says and believes …

Managing Ethically Cultural Diversity: Learning
from Thomas Aquinas

João César das Neves • Domènec Melé

Received: 22 September 2012 / Accepted: 1 July 2013 / Published online: 3 August 2013

� Springer Science+Business Media Dordrecht 2013

Abstract Cultural diversity is an inescapable reality and

a concern in many businesses where it can often raise

ethical questions and dilemmas. This paper aims to offer

suggestions to certain problems facing managers in dealing

with cultural diversity through the inspiration of Thomas

Aquinas. Although he may be perceived as a voice from

the distant past, we can still find in his writings helpful and

original ideas and criteria. He welcomes cultural differ-

ences as a part of the perfection of the universe. His sys-

temic approach leads one to place the problem in its proper

context, and to reflect on it from the perspective of virtue

ethics, with a central role for practical wisdom and giving

primacy to neighborly love and natural moral law. Rather

than a set of rigid standards with no consideration of

diversity Aquinas focuses on the common human ground,

which allows for the indispensable dialogue between dif-

ferent positions. When dealing with practical questions, the

problem is one of finding the right balance between general

principle and cultural specifics, tolerance, and dialogue,

always guided by practical wisdom. In this way, Aquinas’

approach is neither rigid ethical universalism with no

consideration for diversity nor moral relativism with no

place for any transcultural and absolute morals.

Keywords Thomas Aquinas � Cultural diversity � Ethical
dialogue � Managing diversity � Natural law � Virtue ethics

Introduction

Cultural diversity, generally understood as the quality of

diverse or different cultures, often concerns many busi-

nesses. This diversity may typically include differences in

race, ethics, age, gender, religion, and cultural background

though the list of factors reflecting diversity could, in fact,

be wider. In the last few decades, business organizations

have been becoming increasingly diverse and some com-

panies are trying to create multicultural organizations (Cox

1991, 1993, 2001). Cultural diversity appears as a conse-

quence of globalization, and also due to workgroup

diversity in business activities in many places.

Diversity matters regarding competitiveness and per-

formance, as we will see below. Nevertheless, beyond

these goals, although often in connection with it, cultural

diversity posits ethical matters (Gilbert et al. 1999; Noon

2007; Nelson et al. 2012, among others). These include

attitudes toward the ethics of different peoples, the influ-

ence of diversity in making moral judgments, the possi-

bility of reaching basic agreements, universalism and

cultural relativism, dialogue between people of different

cultural background and giving opportunities to histori-

cally disadvantaged groups. Although, some ethical theo-

ries have been applied to these problems, as we also

discuss below, we are far from having a convincing theory

to deal with such dilemmas, which are often very

complex.

The aim of this paper is to contribute to managing

diversity ethically by an exploration of some insights of

Thomas Aquinas–Saint Thomas for the Catholic Church–,

one of the outstanding Scholastic thinkers (Melé 2013). We

place special emphasis on his thought in the realms of

natural moral law and practical rationality –closely related

to the virtue of practical wisdom.

J. C. das Neves (&)
CLSBE Universidade Catolica Portuguesa, Palma de Cima,

1649-023 Lisbon, Portugal

e-mail: [email protected]

D. Melé

IESE Business School Av Pearson, 21, 08034 Barcelona, Spain

e-mail: [email protected]

123

J Bus Ethics (2013) 116:769–780

DOI 10.1007/s10551-013-1820-1

The choice of this 13th century philosopher and theo-

logian may seem awkward, as he lived more than 600 years

before the birth of cultural anthropology, and his approach

to the questions of ethics on cultural diversity is very dif-

ferent from those which tend to be used in our globalized

era. We are, of course, aware that there is a large gap

between contemporaneous conditions and those existing

some 700 years ago, but we assume that if we attempt to

bridge this chasm, we may make valuable discoveries and

identify important ideas. Underlying Aquinas’s writings

there is a certain cultural philosophy, albeit this may only

extend to ‘‘the deduction, demonstration and criticism of

the values and goods of culture according to the meta-

physical and theological principles and guidelines of his

system.’’ (Grabmann 1925, p. 37)

It should be stressed here that this is not a first attempt to

consider Aquinas in business ethics. His influence was

clear in early business ethics (Melé 1999; Wren 2000;

Alves and Moreira 2010; Schlag 2013) from the 14th to

17th centuries, and he is still significant nowadays (das

Neves 2008; Melé 2013; Alford 2013). Several scholars

have applied Aquinas’ thought to specific topics of busi-

ness and management, such as motivation (Llano 1991;

Schoengrund 1996), wealth creation (das Neves 2000), just

price (Friedman 1980; Koehn and Wilbratte 2012, see also

Elegido 2009), social responsibility of business (Wishloff

2009), decision-making (Velasquez and Brandy 1997;

Grassl 2010), just wage (Frémeaux and Noël 2011), and

justice for global business (Dierksmeier and Celano 2012),

among others.

The paper is structured as follows. Firstly, we will try to

identify the nature of some questions of cultural diversity

which arise in business ethics. Secondly, we will present

some insights from Aquinas’ on cultural diversity. Thirdly,

we will discuss some relevant aspects of Thomas Aquinas’

ethics and, in the following section, their significance in

dealing with diversity. Finally, we will present some

practical suggestions taken from Aquinas for an intercul-

tural dialogue.

The Challenge of Cultural Diversity

Cultural diversity presents the practical consequence that

people from different cultural backgrounds may have dif-

ferent belief structures, priorities, perceptions, assumptions

about future events, beliefs about the role of information, and

information-processing methods as Pieterse et al. (2013,

p. 784) pointed out, mentioning a review from several

authors (Cox and Blake 1991; Ely and Thomas 2001; Maz-

nevski 1994; Tsui and O’Reilly 1989; and others). This

indicates that cultural diversity has an impact on organiza-

tional groups (Milliken and Martins 1996; Williams and

O’Reilly 1998, with a review of 40 years of research) and

implications in organizing work (Ely and Thomas 2001; van

Knippenberg and Schippers 2007), in managing derived

conflicts (Pelled et al. 1999) and in retaining talented

minorities within organizations (Thomas and Gabarro 1999).

Cultural diversity can also have an influence on orga-

nizational competitiveness (Cox and Blake 1991; DiRienzo

et al. 2007) and performance (Cox 1991, 2001; Milliken

and Martins 1996; Thomas and Ely 1996; Williams and

O’Reilly 1998; Pelled et al. 1999; Kochan et al. 2003;

Jackson et al. 2003, with a review of previous research on

the influence of organizational diversity on team perfor-

mance; Joshi and Roh 2009; McMahon 2010; Pieterse et al.

2013). Cultural diversity can be also related to human

capital. Martı́n-Alcázar et al. (2012) suggests the notion of

‘human capital diversity’ to refer to a construct formed by

demographic attributes (age, gender, nationality, education,

kind of training, tenure, and functional experience) and

human capital attributes (knowledge, experiences, cogni-

tive styles, and values).

At the root of cultural diversity are certain worldviews,

sociocultural heritages, norms, and values shared by

members of one cultural identity (Cox 1993; Ely and

Thomas 2001; Worchel 2005). From ancient times, such

diversity has posited difficult questions. A classical pre-

sentation of cultural diversity, which offers different

judgments and suggests mutual cultural respect, is the

experience of Persian king Darius, as related by Herodotus:

When he [Darius] was king of Persia, he summoned

the Greeks who happened to be present at his court,

and asked them what they would take to eat the dead

bodies of their fathers. They replied that they would

not do it for any money in the world. Later, in the

presence of the Greeks, and through an interpreter, so

they could understand what was said, he asked some

Indians, of the tribe called Calltie, who do in fact eat

their parent’s dead bodies, what they would take to

burn them. They uttered a cry of horror and forbade

him to mention such a dreadful thing. So firmly

rooted are these beliefs; and it is, I think, rightly said

in Pindar’s poem that custom is lord of all. (Herod-

otus 1996, III, 38, pp. 219–220)

Nowadays, cultural diversity challenges many business

firms, especially those which operate in different countries

or have employees with a variety of beliefs, backgrounds,

styles, ages, language, religions and customs. When

considering the clash between different customs at the

firm or market level, it is easy to fall at either of the

extremes, postulating a universal ethics, excluding any

cultural diversity or a cultural relativism, rejecting any

universal principle (Donaldson 1996). Both of these

positions eliminate the question without solving it. This

770 J. C. das Neves, D. Melé

123

seems akin to a certain often-raised issue in economic

theory of the markets. The extreme models of monopoly

and perfect competitions are the easier to solve, but also the

less relevant. Facing a debate encompassing diverse

cultural elements, one must search for a solution including

both the common ground and the diversity of elements

present.
1

Some companies, however, at least in their corporate

statements, show that it is possible to harmonize cultural

diversity with universal ethical principles. This is the case

of two well-known companies which accept both cultural

diversity and universal ethical principles. One is Deloite, a

firm of professional services with 195,000 professionals

and operating in more than 150 countries, which not only

accepts cultural diversity but also stresses that it is a source

of success. Thus, its ‘‘strength from cultural diversity’’ is

presented as one of its main corporate shared values
2

, and

in its 2011 Annual Review makes the self-observation that

‘‘Talented people choose to work where their differences

are respected and they have access to the opportunities to

realize their potential. Diversity is a Deloitte core value—

an intentional part of talent and business decisions; a driver

of innovation and opportunity; and a strength.’’ (Deloitte

2011, p. 12) This emphasis on diversity does not prevent a

strong commitment to ethical principles, including honesty,

compliance with the law, competence, confidentiality,

integrity, objectivity, fair business practices, respect and

fair treatment (Deloite, Ethics & Compliance, s.d).

The second example regards the multinational oil

company, Shell. On one hand, this company adopted a set

of principles (‘‘Shell General Business Principles’’, SGBP),

some of which are related to certain cultural practices,

including dilemmas related to gift and hospitality practices

in certain countries and bribery. The third principle on

‘‘Business Integrity’’ states: ‘‘Shell companies insist on

honesty, integrity and fairness in all aspects of our business

and expect the same in our relationships with all those with

whom we do business. The direct or indirect offer, pay-

ment, soliciting or acceptance of bribes in any form is

unacceptable. Facilitation payments are also bribes and

should not be made.’’ (Royal Dutch Shell plc. 2006) On the

other hand, this company recognizes diversity in taking

into account that some cultural practices should not be

considered as bribes, as a rigid observer might say:

‘‘Understand local customs for the giving or receiving of

gifts, payments, entertainment or benefits. Customs

regarding tips and fees differ depending on the culture.

When a tip is customary and is fair reward for a genuine

service, then that is acceptable. Tips are given after the

service has been received not before. Adaptation to local

customs is not acceptable when this leads to acting in conflict

with the SGBP’’ (Royal Dutch Shell plc. 2003, p. 18)

These two examples, however, do not mean that har-

monizing universal values with cultural diversity is always

an easy exercise, as noted by Donaldson:

When we leave home and cross our nation’s bound-

aries, moral clarity often blurs. Without a backdrop of

shared attitudes, and without familiar laws and judi-

cial procedures that define standards of ethical con-

duct, certainty is elusive. (1996, p. 48)

Definitively, management faces many challenges regarding

cultural diversity, and many companies have introduced

policies and practices to solve these. However, some

findings show that desirable benefits, such as reduction of

turn-over among talented people from minority groups, the

improvement of the quality of life at work or the creation

of an atmosphere of inclusion are often not achieved (Pless

and Maak 2004, mentioning some studies). Considering

this fact, Pless and Maak (2004, p. 130) suggest building a

culture that ‘‘embraces diversity and fosters humanity’’.

We now turn to what we can learn from Thomas

Aquinas in this regard and in other relevant aspects of

managing cultural diversity.

Aquinas’ Insights on Cultural Diversity

Sympathy for Diversity

Several insights on cultural diversity can be found in

Aquinas’ writings. The first is his sympathy for diversity,

which comes from both his faith and from the rational

consideration of the richness of diversity. He argues that

diversity among creatures was necessary in order that ‘‘the

divine goodness might the more perfectly be bestowed on

things’’ and adds ‘‘there should be diversity among them,

so that what could not be perfectly represented by one

single thing, might be more perfectly represented in various

ways by things of various kinds.’’ (Aquinas 1997, III, 97)

The degree of goodness is not the same in everything:

‘‘Perfect goodness would not be found in things, unless

there were degrees of goodness, so that, to wit, there be

some things better than others: else all the possible degrees

of goodness would not be fulfilled, nor would any creature

be found like to God in the point of being better than

others.’’ (Aquinas 1997, III, 71) It is interesting to add that

he connects this with beauty, and even sees here a justifi-

cation for the presence of evil in the universe. He affirms

that the beauty of the universe ‘‘results from the ordered

1
This may be seen as an application of the motto Alfred Marshall

used in his 1919 classic Industry and Trade: ‘‘The many in the one,

the one in the many’’ (Marshall 1919, p. 1).
2

See, e.g., Code of Conduct of Canada Deloite: http://www.deloitte.

com/assets/Dcom-Canada/Local%20Assets/Documents/Code%20of%

20Conduct.pdf Accessed on January 31, 2013.

Managing Ethically Cultural Diversity 771

123

unity of good and evil things, seeing that evil arises from

the lack of good, and yet certain goods are occasioned from

those very evils through the providence of the governor,

even as the silent pause gives sweetness to the chant.’’

(Aquinas 1997, III, 71)

Influence of Culture and Rational Morality

The second pertinent insight regards the influence of cul-

ture and education on moral behavior, on one hand, and

rational morality, on the other. Regarding the former, he

affirms that ‘‘custom, especially if it dates from our

childhood, acquires the force of nature, the result being that

the mind holds those things with which it was imbued from

childhood as firmly as though they were self-evident’’

(Aquinas 1997, I, 11). Actually, this is no great novelty but

an application of the old Aristotelian principle that ‘‘cus-

tom is a second nature’’ pointed out in De Memoria et

Reminiscentia (Aristotle, 1931, chap. 2).

Aquinas, although recognizing the influence of culture

on moral behavior, holds that in spite of such influence a

permanent basic question remains: what is the right thing

for me to do here and now? In order to answer this axio-

logical question, according to Aquinas, it is necessary first

to assert the meaning of the particular situation. Evaluating

specific actions must start by determining their general

sense. Thus, beneath the ethical question there is another

query, which is not ethical in itself, but which is crucial for

the foundation of ethics—it is necessary to know what the

game is before establishing what constitutes a good move

or a good player. Listening to Aquinas and his coherent and

integrated vision of reality allows us to go a step further

and state that the meaning of any specific situation is

connected to the deeper meaning of reality. This leads us to

inquire about the purpose of reality and life, the funda-

mental investigation of human existence. Where did we

come from? Where do we go from here? What is the reason

for life? Is there the Absolute?

From Aquinas’ perspective it is not possible to have a

reasoned and defensible answer to any ethical question

about the right thing to do without a specific answer to the

ontological, anthropological, and metaphysical questions

on the meaning of the activity and the purpose of

behavior. In the philosophical system of Thomas Aquinas

this connection is obvious precisely because it is a system.

The whole structure deducing choice and ethics from

happiness (in the sense of human flourishing) as the ulti-

mate human end is an expression of this relation.
3

This

connects with another old Aristotelian principle, men-

tioned by St Thomas: ‘‘such a man is, such does the end

seem to him’’ (1981 I-II, 58, 5; Aristotle 1934, III, 5).

This raises the need to acquire knowledge of the ultimate

end of the human being and behave in accordance with it.

Both faith and reason, which in Aquinas are in full har-

mony, provide the answer, which comes from both divine

law and rational moral law. The latter, according to

Thomas Aquinas, can be discovered in human nature. We

will return to this point below when dealing with natural

moral law.

Tolerance and Non-Discrimination

A third insight is an attitude of tolerance. Tolerance for St

Thomas and his contemporaries meant respect for other

persons and their ideas, but without showing ambivalence

or weakness in one’s convictions. Similarly, Aquinas is

aware of the importance of avoiding discrimination against

persons (acceptione personarum), since this is a violation

of distributive justice (see Aquinas 1981, II–II, 63).
4

Albeit quite different from topics pertinent nowadays,

we may find useful cues in specific analyses by Aquinas,

since certain controversies in the 13th century are in some

way akin to our current debates. This is the case of a

question his posited in the Treatise of Faith (Aquinas 1981,

II–II, 10, 11), dealing specifically with unbelievers. He asks

whether the rites of unbelievers should be tolerated.

Business ethics is at first sight alien to this inquiry, but, it is

very relevant to today’s economy. It is not difficult to find

examples of immigrant workers practicing their own reli-

gion in a company within a country in which the majority

has a different faith. The attitude recommended by Tho-

mas, accompanied by a justification, is the following:

Human government is derived from the Divine gov-

ernment, and should imitate it. Now although God is

all-powerful and supremely good, nevertheless He

allows certain evils to take place in the universe,

which He might prevent, lest, without them, greater

goods might be forfeited, or greater evils ensue.

Accordingly in human government also, those who

are in authority, rightly tolerate certain evils, lest

certain goods be lost, or certain greater evils be

incurred. (…) Hence, though unbelievers sin in their
rites, they may be tolerated, either on account of

some good that ensues there from, or because of some

evil avoided. (Aquinas 1981, II–II 10, 11)

Notice that the purpose, as always, is the highest good

possible, but this may require accepting some evil.

Again, this general attitude of lenience and open-

mindedness finds its justification in the actions of God

3
See Aquinas 1981, I–II, 1, particularly article 6, on ‘‘Whether man

ordains all to the last end?’’.

4
The general principle here is: ‘‘a just judge regards causes, not

persons.’’ (Aquinas 1981, II-II 63, 1).

772 J. C. das Neves, D. Melé

123

himself.
5

It should be said this tolerance is an application of

an old principle, today mostly ignored in political activism,

which was stated first by Augustine of Hippo and quoted by

St. Thomas: ‘‘human law cannot punish or forbid all evil deeds:

since while aiming at doing away with all evils, it would do

away with many good things’’ (Aquinas 1981, I-II 91, 4)

Relevant Aspects of Thomas Aquinas’ Ethics

Virtues and the Primacy of Love

The moral philosophy of Thomas Aquinas, presented in

several of his works
6

has been studied by many scholars

(among whom we find, Garcı́a-López 1979; McInerny

1982, 1992; Wadell 1991; Westberg 1994; Finnis 1998;

Schockenhoff 2003, Chap. IV; Houser 2004; DeYoung

et al. 2009; in addition to twenty-seven scholars in a col-

lective work edited by Pope 2002). Although Thomas

Aquinas’ ethics is frequently known for his doctrine on

natural law (see below), it is essentially a virtue ethics. He

discusses ethics through virtues, but virtues are not inde-

pendent from the natural moral law. On the contrary,

transgressions of the moral law (sins) are ‘‘contrary to all

the acts of virtue’’ (1981, I-II, 100, 2).

Following Christian tradition, Aquinas’ ethics is a virtue

ethics in which love or charity (charitas) has primacy

(Wadell 1991). In Aquinas’ words, charity (love) is the

form of all virtues. As he explains, ‘‘in morals the form of

an act is taken chiefly from the end’’ (198, I-II, 23, 8); and

ultimately all virtues have love as their true end. This

means that charity directs the acts of all other virtues to the

last end and, consequently, gives the form to all other acts

of virtues. In other words, charity is the efficient cause of

all virtues (Ibidem).

In line with Aquinas, the official teaching of the Roman

Catholic Church points out that the practice of all the

virtues is animated and inspired by charity (love), and

charity binds everything together in perfect harmony; it is

the form of the virtues and articulates and orders them.

Charity is the source and the goal of Christian practice; it

upholds and purifies our human ability to love, and raises it

to the supernatural perfection of divine love (cf. Catholic

Church 2003 n.1827).

Practical Reason

Along with the primacy of love, practical reason is central

in Aquinas’ ethics. He explains what practical reason is by

saying: ‘‘the reason that deals with things to be done for an

end is the practical reason’’ (1981, II–II, 47, 2). In other

texts, he also affirms that practical rationality is intellectual

discernment between good and evil (1981, II–II, 94, 12)

which leads one to knowing the human good and to acting

in accordance with it (1981, II–II, 94, 1, 3). Practical

rationality presupposes we have an innate habit, termed

‘synderesis’, which incites us to seek good and to reject evil,

inasmuch as through first principles we proceed to discover,

and judge what we have discovered (1981, I, 79, 12).

The existence of practical reason and ‘synderesis’ in

every human being is shown by the common experience of

moral discernment between good and evil. This discern-

ment includes certain behaviors. While some of these are

universally understood as good, such as sacrificing oneself

to help another person, the contrary is the case with others,

such as trampling underfoot another person in pursuit of

one’s own interests.

In contrast with theoretical reason which is ‘contem-

plative’, i.e., oriented to the knowledge of the truth and

with no connection to the action, practical reason seeks

‘practical truth’ which makes a choice morally right. In this

sense, Aristotle, who is followed by Aquinas to great

extent, although with certain differences (Celano 2010),

introduced the idea of practical reason, by considering

firstly the role of moral virtues in making (ethically) right

choices. He affirms: ‘‘moral virtue is a disposition of the

mind in regard to choice, and choice is deliberate desire, it

follows that, if the choice is to be good, both the principle

must be true and the desire right, and that desire must

pursue the same things as principle affirms.’’ (1934, VI, 2)

In other words, a correct desire is necessary for good

behavior, but this requires both understanding what is right

and having the will to do it. Then Aristotle adds: ‘‘We are

here speaking of practical thinking, and of the attainment

of truth in regard to action (…) The attainment of truth is
indeed the function of every part of the intellect, but that of

the practical intelligence is the attainment of truth corre-

sponding to right desire.
7
’’ (1934, VI, 2, italics are ours)

There is a specific intellectual virtue, termed prudence

or practical wisdom, which reinforces the capacity of

practical reason. According to Aristotle, practical wisdom

(or prudence) ‘‘is a truth-attaining rational quality, con-

cerned with action in relation to things that are good and

bad for human beings.’’ (1934, VI, 5) Practical wisdom,

does not deal with universals only, but needs to take

5
This may remind us of the necessity to imitate the patience of the

Divine Providence, which ‘‘causes his sun to rise on the evil and the

good, and sends rain on the righteous and the unrighteous.’’ (Bible,

Matthew 5: 45).
6

Particularly, Commentary on Aristotle’s Nicomachean Ethics (In

decem libros Ethicorum expositio) (Aquinas 1993 [1271–1272]);

Summa Theologica (Summa Theologiae) (1981, 2nd part [1273]);

Summa contra gentiles (1997, 3
rd

part [1261–1263]).

7
That is truth about the means to the attainment of the rightly-desired

End (note in the Rackham’s translation of Nicomachean Ethics).

Managing Ethically Cultural Diversity 773

123

cognizance of singulars also (Aristotle 1934, VI, 7).

Similarly, Aquinas affirms that practical wisdom or pru-

dence as ‘‘right reason in action’’ (1981, II–II, 47, 2), or

‘‘right reason about things to be done’’ (1981, I-II, 58, 3).

In other words, in both Aquinas and Aristotle, prudence

‘‘represents the agent’s ability to deliberate, decide and

properly to order the process of practical reason to

action.’’ (Westberg 1994, p. 187) Practical wisdom rein-

forces practical reason to discern in each particular situ-

ation what is truly good and to choose the right means of

achieving such good.

Practical wisdom also acts as a driver of all moral vir-

tues. Acting with generosity, for instance, requires deter-

mining what action means being generous in a given

situation. Such determination requires practical wisdom

(prudence). As Aquinas affirms, ‘‘choice of the means is

the concern of prudence.’’ (Aquinas 1981, I-II, 58, 3, 1)

This is in line with Aristotle who said ‘‘true virtue cannot

exist without Prudence’’ (Aristotle 1934, VI, 13), and

‘‘virtue ensures the rightness of the end we aim at, Pru-

dence ensures the rightness of the means we adopt to gain

that end.’’ (Aristotle 1934, VI, 12)

Natural Moral Law

Virtues, animated and inspired by love, and practical wis-

dom converge into natural moral law. Practical rationality

discovers human good as something which should be done.

According to Aquinas, ‘‘good is the first thing that falls

under the apprehension of practical reason, which is

directed to action: since every agent acts for an end under

the aspect of good.’’ (1981, I-II 94, 2) This leads him to

enunciate the first principle of natural moral law, which is

founded on the notion of good, and expresses as a funda-

mental ethical duty inherent to good. ‘‘The first precept …

International Journal of Arts & Sciences,

CD-ROM. ISSN: 1944-6934 :: 09(02):135–144 (2016)

CULTURAL DIFFERENCES ON CONFLICT STRATEGIES IN THE

WORKPLACE

Vignesh Murugavel and Ajay Somaraju

University of Texas at Dallas, United States of America

Studies have established distinct cultural preferences for resolving workplace conflict; however, few

studies examine these cross-cultural differences within the context of a common host country. This

study proposes to abridge the gap in research by exploring conflict strategy preferences of South Asian,

East Asian, and Western cultures within the United States. Putnam and Wilson’s (1982) Organizational

Communication Conflict Instrument was distributed with a demographic sheet across 312 working age

individuals. Responses from individuals brought up in South Asian (n=95), East Asian (n=88), and

Western (n=89) cultures were grouped and analyzed, respectively. Findings revealed that East Asians,

South Asians, and Westerners preferred compromising/collaborative strategies over the alternatives. In

addition, East Asians were more likely to use controlling strategies than Westerners, Westerners were

more likely to use compromising/collaborative strategies than East Asians, and South Asians’ responses

resembled Westerners’ more than to East Asians’. Finally, westernization did not predict foreign

individuals’ conflict styles, suggesting that individuals did not simply conform to host country norms.

These results provide evidence for foreign individuals resolving disagreements differently within a host

country than within their heritage country and hold implications for the applicability of Hofstede’s

Individualism-Collectivism paradigm within a multinational company.

Keywords: Culture, Conflict, Organizational psychology, Applied psychology.

Introduction

Conflict

Organizational conflict refers to the exchange of opposing viewpoints within the workplace. Role

relationships influence organizational conflict through coworker-coworker and subordinate-superior

interactions (Khan et. al, 1964; Holt & DeVore, 2005; Putnam & Wilson, 1982). All cultures do not

experience these role relationships in the same manner (Hofstede, 1984), leading to an increased potential

for inter-organizational conflict in multinational companies (MNCs).

Not only should managers/leaders of MNCs resolve such conflict, they should also adjust their

approach to handling these differences across cultures. Approaching conflict resolution requires an

evaluation of employee conflict styles. Regarded as the first researchers to establish a conflict measure,

Blake and Mouton (1964) created a trait based dimensional model that examines individual conflict styles

(Putnam and Wilson, 1982). Blake and Mouton (1970) describe these dimensions as concerns for people

or results, from which they derive five distinct styles of conflict management.

135

136 Cultural Differences on Conflict Strategies in the Workplace

Though subsequent studies’ findings converge to provide the basis for a dimensional model of

conflict, problems exist in their methodology. As these studies follow a trait based model, they imply a

predisposition of the individual towards using a certain conflict style. Determining this predisposition

may not prove useful. Though the measurement may find that individuals innately prefer one conflict

style over the others, the context of a given conflict can force these individuals to utilize a style that they

do not inherently prefer to achieve a resolution. In addition, an individual may score highly on as many as

three different styles (Putnam & Wilson, 1982). Therefore, though individuals may carry the strongest

disposition towards a certain conflict style, they may also strongly tend to use another conflict style. This

may provide conflicting information as some styles contradict each other. All of these deficiencies in

conflict studies lend themselves to faults in reliability and validity.

In order to address confounds in Blake and Mouton’s (1964) trait-based model, Putnam and Wilson

(1982) created the Organizational Communication Conflict Instrument (OCCI). The OCCI splits conflict

preferences into three strategies: control, non-confrontation, solution-orientation. Utilizing a

strategic/situational paradigm that emphasizes the context of the situation over immutable traits, the OCCI

yields higher test-retest reliability and internal consistencies than almost all other measures of conflict

communication (Wilson & Waltman, 1988). Thus, the OCCI provides for strong “real-world” application

when evaluating employee conflict strategies.

Culture

The effect of culture on workplace behavior is widely documented (e.g. Corey, Fok, and Payne, 2014;

Chang, 2002; Lee & Rogan, 1991). Researchers have often used Hofstede’s cultural dimensions to

categorize the effect of culture on workplace behavior: power distance, individualism-collectivism,

masculinity-femininity, uncertainty avoidance, long-term orientation, and indulgence (Hofstede, 1984;

1991; Hofstede & Bond, 1988). In particular, individualism-collectivism refers to an individual’s cultural

view on the importance of the individual versus the group. Individualistic cultures tend to view members

as autonomous and loosely connected, while tightly knit social frameworks characterize collectivistic

cultures (Song, Yong-Jin, 2004). The individualism-collectivism dimension accounts for the largest

difference in employee workplace behavior (Lee & Rogan, 1991), leading most cross-cultural research to

examine this dimension in comparative studies (Song, Yong-Jin, 2004).

Research does not universally support this dimension as the explanation for behavioral differences

between cultures. Other theories that propose cultural values as a more salient element than the

individualist-collectivist dimension have empirical support as well (Silverthorne, 2005). Morris et al.

(1998), Bond & Wang (1983), Chow & Ding (2001), and Tse, Francis, & Walls (1994) posit that

pervasive cultural values like self-enhancement, long-term relationships, and conformity underlie conflict

strategy usage. Consolidating these independent theories requires further research and theoretical

development of workplace and cultural constructs.

Understanding cross-cultural organizational conflict necessitates recognition of conflict strategies

within the context of both heritage and host country cultural values. Current research often focuses on the

former (e.g. Ohbuchi and Takashi, 1994; Chiu and Kosinski, 1994; Corey, Fok, & Payne, 2014), while

downplaying the latter; therefore, research does not often address the effect of a host country’s cultural

values on an individual’s conflict strategy. Knowing foreign individuals’ conflict strategies in their

heritage country may not predict their actions in a host country. For example, determining the effect of

collectivism or conformity on a Chinese employee is not enough if only limited to the scope of work

situations in China. These effects are possibly inapplicable when this Chinese employee immigrates to a

Western country and interacts with the unique cultural values of the West. This Chinese employee, who

may have consistently used an avoidance based conflict strategy during disagreements with a Chinese

superior, could change communication methods based upon exposure to Western ideals and norms.

As MNCs increase foreign hires, effectively resolving workplace conflicts with people of different

cultural backgrounds within a common country is paramount.

Vignesh Murugavel and Ajay Somaraju 137

This study proposes to examine the relationships between conflict strategies and cultural differences.

Specifically, we explore strategy usage and cultural differences of South and East Asian populations

within the United States. Based on past research, there are several assumptions we can make. The goal of

this study is to determine whether the following assumptions, based on findings from individuals’

heritage countries, will hold in a common host country (U.S.). If the aforementioned findings do not

generalize, we predict Westernization explains differences between individuals’ behavior in host and

heritage countries.

Hypotheses

The control strategy involves insistently pursuing self-interests and demonstrating a domineering

disposition during resolution formation. Westerners, particularly US citizens, often practice such

“competitive and adversarial” strategies during conflict resolution (Li, Cheung, and Kau, 1979; Leung

and Lind, 1986). Therefore, we expect Western participants in our study to utilize the control strategy

more than the alternatives. Furthermore, we expect Westerners to utilize the control strategy more than

East Asian participants.

Westernization is the process by which foreign individuals modify their behavior and/or values to

include traditionally Western principles. Morris et. al (1998) proposes that shared socialization leads to

behavioral social conformity; therefore, foreign populations in the US should converge towards western

ideals over time. Studies purporting Westernization affecting managerial values (e.g. Lin, 1995) provide

evidence for this phenomenon. We expect Westernization will predict foreign individuals’ control

strategy usage scores.

The non-confrontation strategy involves shying away from direct disagreements, resolutions

involving this style develop through indirect communication. East Asian culture emphasizes this

communication style through creating a deferential atmosphere and interpreting direct confrontation by a

subordinate as shots to management’s authority (Tjosvold and Sun, 2000). This evidence, along with

documented preference of avoidance strategies by Chinese workers (Bond and Wang, 1983), leads us to

predict that those from East Asian cultures will utilize the non-confrontation strategy more than the

alternatives. Furthermore, we expect East Asians to utilize the non-confrontation strategy more than

Western participants.

Since South Asian and East Asian cultures are both considered highly collectivist and Western

cultures are considered highly individualist (Hofstede, 1984), we predict that South Asian scores will not

significantly differ from East Asians’ scores across strategies. Conversely, we predict that South Asian

scores will significantly differ from Western scores on all strategies. Furthermore, we also predict that

East Asian scores will significantly differ from Western scores on all strategies.

Methodology

Sample

Data were collected using a two portion self-reported survey. The first portion of the survey consisted of a

demographic sheet that evaluated age, gender, ethnicity, cultural background, and years spent in the US.

The second portion was a modified OCCI (Wilson & Waltman, 1988). The questionnaires were

administered to participants using convenience sampling through various social media outlets on the

Internet; all respondents lived in the US. There was no time limit for completing this survey.

Out of the 312 questionnaires, 291 were usable, as incomplete responses were discarded. Out of the

remaining questionnaires, 19 were unused because they did not fit the demographic of interest. The age

range of the participants varied widely from 18 to 71 (N=262; SD=9.29). There were 183 (63.1%) male

participants and 107 (36.9%) female participants. The cultural distribution of studied participants was

138 Cultural Differences on Conflict Strategies in the Workplace

relatively equal among East Asian (N=88, 30.2%), Western (N=89, 30.6%), and South Asian (N=95,

32.6%) cultures.

Organizational Communication Conflict Instrument (OCCI)

The Organizational Communication Conflict Instrument (OCCI) (Putnam & Wilson, 1982) reliably

measures an individual’s conflict style within the construct of three distinct strategies. The OCCI contains

35 items that measure conflict strategy usage on a 7-point Likert scale. These items refer to workplace

scenarios within the context of approaching conflict with a supervisor. Our study borrowed Wilson and

Waltman’s (1988) modified version of the OCCI. This instrument is comprised of 30 items from Putnam

& Wilson’s original Form B that load significantly higher on one strategy. The amount of usage of a

certain strategy is coded by using the formula observed in Wilson and Waltman (1988).

The conflict strategies were tapped using the following questions from Wilson and Waltman (1988).

Solution-orientation: Questions 1,4,6,8,9,11,13,16,19,20,21

Non-confrontation: Questions 2,5,7,12,14,15,23,24,25,27,28,29

Control: Questions 30,26,22,17,18,10,3

Sample items included:

* “I make my opinion known in a disagreement with my supervisor”

* “I steer clear of disagreeable situations”

* “I suggest solutions which combine a variety of viewpoints”

Results

Within-Subjects Effects

To test the hypothesis that Westerners would have lower control strategy scores compared to their scores

for solution-orientation and non-confrontation strategies, a repeated measures ANOVA was conducted on

our Western demographic sample on their strategy scores across all three strategies. Results revealed

significant main effect of strategy indicating differences between the three strategies, F(2, 176) = 69.77,

p < .001, 2= .44. A follow-up pairwise comparison indicated that participants had significantly lower

solution-orientation strategy scores (M = 3.19, SD = .08) compared to all other strategy scores (non-

confrontation, M = 4.30, SD = .10; control, M = 4.72, SD = .09), all ps < .001. Also, participant’s non-

confrontation strategy scores (M = 4.30, SD = .10) were significantly lower than their control strategy

scores (M = 4.72, SD = .09), p = .006. Remembering that lower scores indicate increased strategy usage,

these results are inconsistent with our hypothesis, Westerners actually used the solution-orientation

strategy more than the control or non-confrontation strategies. It is also interesting to note that Westerners

used control strategy the least compared to the solution-orientation and non-confrontation strategies.

To test the hypothesis that East Asians would have lower non-confrontation strategy scores

compared to their scores for solution-orientation and control strategies, a repeated measures ANOVA was

conducted on our East Asian demographic sample on their strategy scores across all three strategies.

Results revealed a significant main effect of strategy indicating differences between the three strategies,

F(2, 174) = 27.21, p< .001, 2= .24. A follow-up pairwise comparison indicated that participants had

significantly lower solution-orientation strategy scores (M = 3.69, SD = .07) compared to all other

strategy scores (non-confrontation, M = 4.18, SD = .07; control, M = 4.18, SD = .08), all ps < .001. Also,

participant’s non-confrontation strategy scores (M = 4.18, SD = .07) were not significantly different than

their control strategy scores (M = 4.18, SD = .08), p = 1.00. Remembering that lower scores indicate

increased strategy usage, these results are inconsistent with our hypothesis, East Asians actually used the

solution-orientation strategy more than the non-confrontation or control strategies.

Vignesh Murugavel and Ajay Somaraju 139

A repeated measures ANOVA was conducted on our South Asian demographic sample on their

strategy scores across all three strategies. Results revealed significant main effect of strategy indicating

differences between the three strategies, F(2, 188) = 65.57, p< .001, ^2= .41. A follow-up pairwise

comparison indicated that participants had significantly lower solution-orientation strategy scores

(M = 3.26, SD = .09) compared to all other strategy scores (non-confrontation, M = 4.32, SD = .09;

control, M = 4.41, SD = .10), all ps < .001. Also, participant’s non-confrontation strategy scores

(M = 4.32, SD = .09) were not significantly different than their control strategy scores (M = 4.41,

SD = .10), p = .40. Remembering that lower scores indicate increased strategy usage, these results reveal

that South Asians used the solution-orientation strategy more than the non-confrontation or control

strategies.

Between-Subjects Effects

To test the hypotheses that East Asian scores will significantly differ from Western scores across all

strategies, East Asians would utilize the non-confrontation strategy more than Westerners, Westerners

would utilize the control strategy more than East Asians, and South Asian and East Asian responses will

resemble each other on more strategies than South Asian and Western responses will resemble each other,

a multivariate ANOVA was conducted with the three participant cultures (Western, N = 89; South Asian,

N = 95; East Asian, N = 88) as independent variables and the three conflict strategies (solution-

orientation, non-confrontation, and control) as dependent variables.

Results exposed a significant main effect of culture on strategy usage in general, F(6, 536) = 9.08,

p < .001, 2= .09 ,indicating culture has some effect on how our participants approached conflict.

Specifically, results revealed a significant main effect of culture on solution-orientation strategy scores,

F(2, 269) = 11.68, p < .001, 2= .08; no main effect of culture on non-confrontation strategy scores,

F(2, 269) = .76, p = .47, 2= .01; and, a significant main effect of culture on control strategy usage, F(2,

269) = 8.47, p < .001, 2= .06. This indicates that cultural differences influence solution-orientation and

control strategy usage but not non-confrontation strategy usage.

A stringent posthoc analysis was conducted to further explore results. A Games-Howell Post hoc

analysis revealed that East Asians, M = 3.69, SD = .08, significantly differ from Westerners, M = 3.19,

SD = .08, on their solution-orientation strategy scores, p < .000, and control strategy scores, (East Asian,

M = 4.18, SD = .09; Westerners, M = 4.72, SD = .09) p < .000; however, East Asians, M = 4.18,

SD = .09, do not significantly differ from Westerners, M = 4.30, SD = .09 on their non-confrontation

strategy scores, p = .59. These findings do not support our hypotheses that East Asians would utilize the

non-confrontation strategy more than the Westerners; In fact, East Asians and Westerners did not differ in

their use of the non-confrontation strategy. Furthermore, although the comparison revealed that that East

Asians significantly differ from Westerners on their control strategy scores, this finding supports our

hypothesis that Westerners would utilize the control strategy more than East Asians. Remembering that

lower scores indicate increased strategy usage, the means (Westerners, M = 4.72, SD = .09; East Asian,

M = 4.18, SD = .09; p < .000) reveal that Westerners actually use control strategy less than East Asian.

The analysis further reveals that South Asians, M = 3.26, SD = .08, significantly differ from East

Asians, M = 3.69, SD = .08, on their solution orientation usage; however, South Asians M = 4.32,

SD = .09, and East Asians, M = 4.18, SD = .09, do not significantly differ on non-confrontation strategy

usage, p = .43, and control strategy usage, (South Asian, M = 4.41, SD = .09; East Asian, M = 4.18,

SD = .09) p = .17. These findings indicate that South Asians responses resemble East Asian responses on

two strategies.

Additionally, results from the analysis reveal that South Asians do not significantly differ from

Westerners any of the three strategies. South Asian, M = 3.26, SD = .08, and Westerners, M = 3.19,

SD = .08, do not significantly differ on solution orientation strategy usage, p = .80. South Asian,

M = 4.32, SD = .09, and Westerners, M = 4.30, SD = .09, do not significantly differ on non-confrontation

strategy usage, p = .99. Finally, South Asian, M = 4.41, SD = .09 and Westerners, M = 4.72, SD = .09,

140 Cultural Differences on Conflict Strategies in the Workplace

only trend toward significantly differing on control strategy usage, p = .07. These findings indicate that

South Asians scores resemble Western scores on all three strategies.

In conjunction, the above two findings indicate that South Asian responses resemble Westerner

responses on conflict strategy usage more than they resemble East Asians on conflict strategy usage. This

conclusion does not support our hypothesis that South Asian and East Asian responses will resemble each

other on more strategies than South Asian and Western responses will resemble each other; in actuality

South Asian responses resemble Western responses on more strategies than East Asian responses.

To determine if sex difference affected our results, a co-varied multivariate ANOVA was conducted

with the three participant cultures and sex (male, N = 169; female, N = 102) as independent variables and

the three conflict strategies scores as dependent variables. Results revealed a significant interaction of sex

and culture on strategy usage in general, F(6, 528) = 2.82, p = .01, 2 = .03. However, there were no

significant interactions on each of the strategies. There was a trend towards a significant interaction of sex

and culture on control strategy usage, F(2, 265) = 2.79, p = .06, 2 = .02 and solution orientation strategy

usage, F(2, 265) = 3.00, p = .05, 2 = .02; but no significant interaction of sex and culture on non-

confrontation strategy usage, F(2, 265) = 2.32, p = .10, 2 = .02. These trends toward significance

warranted running this additional multivariate ANOVA to see any changes in significance in strategy

usage between cultures from our initial MANOVA. The co-varied results from the pairwise comparisons

of the test only produce new relationships between control strategy usage and culture. The comparison

now reveals that South Asians, M = 4.41, SD = .09, significantly differ from East Asians, M = 4.18,

SD = .09, p = .01; and South Asians’ scores significantly differ from Westerners’, M = 4.72, SD = .09,

p = .02 on control strategy. These findings now showing that South Asians only resemble East Asians on

one strategy (non-confrontation) and South Asians resemble Westerners on two strategies (solution

orientation and non-confrontation) once co-varied with sex.

The general interaction between sex and culture on strategy usage and trends towards interaction on

specific strategies implies that sex plays some role in influencing cultural differences on communication

strategies during a conflict. Though the effect of sex on our study merited consideration, even when

accounting for the changes that occurred when results were co-varied, our general findings remain

constant. East Asians still reported using the control strategy more often than Westerners, East Asians’

scores were not different than Westerners’ on non-confrontation strategy, and South Asians’ responses still

resembled Westerners’ on conflict strategy usage more than they resembled East Asians’ on conflict

strategy usage.

Time spent in the U.S.

To test the hypothesis that time spent in the U.S would predict control strategy usage in our South Asian

and East Asian sample two linear regression were run on time spent in the U.S. with control strategy

usage as the dependent variable for our South Asian sample and East Asian sample. The results of the

linear regression for our East Asian sample revealed that time spent in the U.S. (M = 3.20, SD = 4.23) did

not predict control strategy usage (M = 4.17, SD = .77), = .129, t(70) = 1.09, p = .28. Also, time spent in

the U.S. did not explain a significant portion of the variance in control strategy usage, R2= .02, F(1, 70) =

1.92, p = .28, in our East Asian sample. The results of the linear regression for our South Asian sample

revealed that time spent in the U.S. (M = 10.92, SD = 10.55) did not predict control strategy usage

(M = 4.49, SD = 1.03), = .129, t(68) = -.21, p = .84. In addition, time spent in the U.S. did not explain a

significant portion of variance in control strategy usage R2= .001, F(1, 68) = .04, p = .84, in our South

Asian sample. These results do not support our hypothesis that time spent in the U.S. would predict

control strategy usage in our South Asian and East Asian sample; in fact, time spent in the U.S. did not

have any predictive power on control strategy usage.

Vignesh Murugavel and Ajay Somaraju 141

Discussion

This study provides further confirmation of the effect of culture on conflict resolution. Although the

general trend found culture having an effect on behavior, this study found several inconsistencies with

current research that also suggests this.

Inconsistencies with Current Research

As the OCCI measures conflict approach through the context of a disagreement with a supervisor,

previous research indicates that East Asians would prefer a deferential, non-confrontational strategy

emphasizing the authority of management over subordinates. Contrary to previous research, our study

found that the East Asian demographic group preferred a solution-oriented approach over a non-

confrontation approach. Analyses comparing strategy usage between cultures further support this

inconsistency. Westerners and South Asians were just as likely as East Asians to use the non-

confrontation strategy. This implies that East Asians within the U.S. may not avoid disagreement or

conflict any more than any other culture.

Research describes Western cultures, the U.S. in particular, as more likely to use adversarial and

competitive techniques in communication than East Asians (Li, Cheung, and Kau, 1979; Leung and Lind,

1986). Since dominating an argument and ‘forcing’ behaviors characterize the control strategy (Putnam

& Wilson, 1982), research would predict a stronger control profile for Westerners compared to East

Asians. This study found that East Asians were actually more likely to use the control strategy than

Westerners. This suggests that East Asians are less avoidant and Westerners are less controlling than

previous research predicts. Analysis of strategy usage within the Western culture supports these findings

and points to a more collaborative picture of Western communication, revealing a stronger preference for

solution-orientation than control strategy.

Researchers often explore cultural tendencies within a heritage country (e.g. South Asian evaluation

within South Asia). This can lead organizations to overgeneralize researchers’ findings to all individuals

from that heritage country regardless of the individuals’ current context. For example, South Asians in a

host country (ex. U.S., China, etc.) would be expected to behave the same as South Asians within South

Asia. The largest discrepancy between the current study and previous research is the evaluation of culture

and conflict within the context of a host country rather than the heritage country. We propose that a

fundamental difference between individuals’ behavior in their heritage country and individuals’ behavior

in a host country exists and accounts for the inconsistencies we observed.

Implications for the Individualism-Collectivism Dimension

Previous research often uses the Individualism-Collectivism (I-C) dimension defined by Hofstede to

account for variations within workplace behavior (Song, Yong-Jin, 2004). Results from this study suggest

that using the I-C dimension to account for behavior may be misguided and that the model itself may not

apply to foreign populations within a host country. Collectivist cultures often use avoidance based

strategies to maintain an air of deference and respect. In contrast, research describes individualist cultures

as normalizing, if not encouraging, subordinate-supervisor disagreements (Song, Yong-Jin, 2004). From

the I-C perspective, our study yields contradictory results. South Asians and East Asians both use the

solution-orientation strategy more often than non-confrontation. South Asians and East Asians are more

likely to use the control strategy than Western cultures. Therefore, collectivist cultures in our study

preferred direct confrontation strategies just as much, if not more than, individualist cultures.

Although South Asian culture emphasizes collectivist ideals, South Asian responses more closely

resemble Western responses than East Asian responses; South Asian responses did not differ from

Westerners’ on any strategy but differed with East Asians’ on one strategy. Under the Individualist scale

(Hofstede, 1984) South Asian cultures fall closer to East Asian cultures than Western cultures.

142 Cultural Differences on Conflict Strategies in the Workplace

The finding that South Asian responses did not differ at all to Westerners’ across all strategies implies

that the I-C dimension may not sufficiently account for behavioral differences. South Asian and East

Asian cultures likely emphasize related but distinct values, causing them to score similarly on blanket

labels like Collectivism but differently on more contextualized measures …

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

A cross-cultural comparison of ethical attitudes of business managers: India, Korea and the Unite…
Christie, P Maria Joseph;Kwon, Ik-Whan G;Stoeberl, Philipp A;Baumhart, Raymond
Journal of Business Ethics; Sep 2003; 46, 3; ProQuest Central
pg. 263

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

error: Content is protected !!