O R I G I N A L P A P E R
Effects of a Perseverative Interest-Based TokenEconomy on Challenging and On-Task Behaviorin a Child with Autism
Amarie Carnett • Tracy Raulston • Russell Lang •
Amy Tostanoski • Allyson Lee • Jeff Sigafoos •
Wendy Machalicek
Published online: 19 March 2014
� Springer Science+Business Media New York 2014
Abstract We compared the effects of a token economy intervention that either didor did not include the perseverative interests of a 7-year-old boy with autism. An
alternating treatment design revealed that the perseverative interest-based tokens
were more effective at decreasing challenging behavior and increasing on-task
behavior than tokens absent the perseverative interest during an early literacy
activity. The beneficial effects were then replicated in the child’s classroom. The
results suggest that perseverative interest-based tokens might enhance the effec-
tiveness of interventions based on token economies.
Keywords Autism � Perseverative interest � Token economy � Challengingbehavior � Alternating treatment design
A. Carnett (&) � J. SigafoosVictoria University of Wellington, Karori Campus, PO Box 17-310, Wellington, New Zealand
e-mail: [email protected]
T. Raulston � W. MachalicekUniversity of Oregon, Eugene, OR, USA
R. Lang � A. LeeClinic for Autism Research Evaluation and Support, Texas State University, San Marcos, TX, USA
R. Lang
Meadows Center for the Prevention of Educational Risk, The University of Texas at Austin, Austin,
TX, USA
A. Tostanoski
Vanderbilt University, Nashville, TN, USA
123
J Behav Educ (2014) 23:368–377
DOI 10.1007/s10864-014-9195-7
Introduction
Token economy interventions involve delivering small tangibles (e.g., tokens)
contingent on the presence or absence of target behaviors and then providing an
opportunity to exchange a preset number of these tokens for backup reinforcers.
Previous research has demonstrated that behaviors can be established, decreased,
and/or maintained using token economy systems (Hackenberg 2009; Matson and
Boisjoli 2009). Research has also investigated several variations of this intervention
including the use of a response cost (i.e., losing tokens for inappropriate behavior),
pairing tokens with praise, and delivering tokens on a variety of intermittent
reinforcement schedules. These variables have been shown to influence the
effectiveness of token economy interventions in some cases (Maggin et al. 2011;
Matson and Boisjoli 2009; Mottram and Berger-Gross 2004).
One aspect of the token economy that has received relatively little attention is the
token itself. Traditionally, tokens are considered to be neutral stimuli (e.g., tickets)
that gain reinforcing power by being paired with the backup reinforcers. Charlop-
Christy and Haymes (1998) investigated the effectiveness of incorporating the
idiosyncratic perseverative interests of children with autism within tokens in an
effort to increase the reinforcing power of the token. Charlop-Christy and Haymes
(1998) defined such intense interests as preoccupations or obsessions that an
individual continually seeks. Results from that study indicated that making use of
tokens that reflected the child’s perseverative interests (e.g., using a small picture of
a train as a token for a child who had a perseverative interest in trains) improved
intervention outcomes. To date, this appears to be the only study to have
demonstrated the potential value of individualizing tokens based on a child’s
perseverative interest.
The purpose of this current study was to replicate and extend the work of
Charlop-Christy and Haymes (1998). Specifically, we compared the effects of a
token economy intervention that either did or did not make use of tokens that
reflected a child’s perseverative interest. We examined the effects of this
manipulation on the challenging and on-task behavior of a 7-year-old boy with
autism during an early literacy activity in a public school special education
classroom and an inclusion classroom.
Method
Participant, Setting, and Materials
Troy was a 7-year-old boy who had been diagnosed with autism. He resided at home
with his father, mother, and three older siblings and attended a local public school.
He scored a 31 on the Childhood Autism Rating Scale (CARS; Schopler et al.
1980), which is indicative of mild-moderate autistic symptoms, and a 99 on the
Behavior Assessment System for Children-II, which indicates an overall clinically
significant range (BASC-II; Reynolds and Kamphaus 2004). Troy spent the majority
of his school day in a special education life skills classroom with four to eight other
J Behav Educ (2014) 23:368–377 369
123
children with developmental disabilities, a special education teacher, and a teaching
assistant. Troy’s individualized education plan (IEP) called for him to spend 1 h of
his school day included in activities with students without disabilities. However,
Troy’s challenging behavior (i.e., screaming, falling, and/or lying on the floor)
occurred too frequently to be acceptable in an inclusion classroom (i.e., a classroom
with a combination of students with and without disability).
The Questions About Behavior Function (QABF) Scale (Matson et al. 2012)
suggested that Troy’s challenging behavior was maintained by escape from
demands. As a result, the inclusion time specified in his IEP was met by
nonacademic activities with fewer demands (e.g., lunch, recess). Troy’s school
counselor referred him to this study in an effort to identify a strategy that could be
used to increase Troy’s inclusion during academic instruction in the general
education classroom. Additionally, Troy had previous experience in using a
traditional token economy within a discrete-trial format, and thus did not require
additional training to use the token economy system for this study.
The baseline and intervention sessions were conducted in Troy’s life skills
classroom and in his inclusion classroom. A video camera on a tripod was used to
record the participant and the researcher during all sessions. The inclusion
classroom included one teacher, 14 students without disabilities, two students with
learning disabilities who spent 100 % of their time in that classroom, and two
students with developmental disabilities who divided their time between the
inclusion and life skills classrooms. Both classrooms had a regularly scheduled early
literacy activity, which lasted 10–12 min and occurred three or four times per week.
During the activity, the teacher sat in a chair and read a story to the children as they
sat on the carpet with a teacher assistant and researcher observing. The children
were expected to sit quietly, look at the teacher or book, listen, and answer
occasional reading comprehension questions.
Response Measurement and Interobserver Agreement
Data were collected on Troy’s challenging behavior and on-task behavior.
Challenging behavior was defined as screaming (i.e., loud vocalizations lasting
3 s or more that were considered disruptive in the classroom), falling, and/or lying
on the ground (i.e., collapsing head and body to the ground). Screaming and falling
often occurred in tandem, and the QABF suggested both were maintained by escape
from demands, so these two topographies, whether they occurred alone or in
combination, were recorded as challenging behavior. On-task behavior was defined
as sitting with buttocks on the ground, head oriented toward the teacher, and having
an absence of challenging behavior. Challenging behavior was scored using 10-s
partial interval recording, and on-task behavior was scored using 10-s whole-
interval recording (Kennedy 2005). The on-task behaviors were selected due to their
incompatibility with Troy’s challenging behavior; thus, challenging behavior and
on-task behavior could not be scored in the same interval. Interval data were
converted to a percentage by dividing the number of intervals with each dependent
variable by the total number of intervals, then multiplying by 100 to convert into a
percentage.
370 J Behav Educ (2014) 23:368–377
123
Data on interobserver agreement (IOA) were collected from videos for both
dependent variables during 30 % of the baseline and intervention sessions by two
trained independent coders. IOA was calculated by dividing the number of intervals
with agreement (i.e., both data collectors scored the presence or absence of
challenging behavior/on-task behavior for the interval) by the total number of
intervals (i.e., agreements plus disagreements), then multiplying by 100 to convert
into a percentage. Mean agreement for both dependent variables was 98.5 % (range
95–100 %).
Treatment integrity was assessed for 30 % of the sessions. A procedural checklist
of intervention procedures (available upon request) was used to record the accuracy
of intervention implementation. The mean of treatment integrity was 96.9 % (range
84.6–100 %).
Procedure
Research Design
The two token economy interventions (i.e., with and without embedded persever-
ative interests) were compared using alternating treatments with an initial baseline
design (Gast 2010). The alternating treatments phase was conducted in the life skills
classroom, and the intervention was implemented by the researcher. Generalization
from the life skills classroom to the inclusion classroom was assessed by conducting
a probe in the inclusion classroom during baseline and by adding a third phase, best-
treatment phase, in which the intervention associated with less challenging behavior
and more on-task behavior was implemented in the inclusion classroom (Gast
2010). Across all phases of the study, the following conditions were held constant:
(a) session duration (10 min), (b) time of day when sessions were conducted, (c) the
types of backup reinforcers that were available, (d) the number and timing of
opportunities to exchange tokens for the backup reinforcers, and (e) the reading
level of the stories. During the intervention and generalization phases, the reading
activity was led by the classroom teacher. A teaching assistant was also present, and
the researcher implemented the intervention.
Baseline
Four of the five baseline sessions were conducted in the life skills classroom. Due to
high rates of challenging behavior, only one baseline session was conducted in the
inclusion classroom. The duration of the reading activity was always between 10
and 12 min. To keep session duration constant, data were recorded during the first
10 min only. During baseline, all teachers and assistants were told to conduct the
reading activity as they would normally. During baseline, the teachers in both
classrooms verbally prompted on-task behavior (e.g., ‘‘Troy, please be quiet and sit
up.’’), provided praise contingent upon on-task behavior, and occasionally ignored
challenging behavior or delivered a mild reprimand (e.g., ‘‘Troy, stop that.’’).
However, none of these components were consistently implemented, and despite
this effort, the participant’s challenging behavior had persisted for over 6 months.
J Behav Educ (2014) 23:368–377 371
123
Preference and Backup Reinforcers
Backup reinforcers were selected by first asking Troy’s teachers to identify potential
reinforcers that would be appropriate in their classrooms. The teachers suggested
small edibles (e.g., bite-sized candy or cracker) because they were inexpensive and
could be consumed quickly without causing distraction. A pairwise preference
assessment was then conducted to identify preference of bite-sized edibles (Fisher
et al. 1992). Prior to each session, Troy selected a backup reinforcer from his top
three preferences (i.e., M&M, fruit snack, and chip). The researcher reviewed on-
task behaviors with Troy using a visual support that included pictures and words of
targeted on-task behaviors (i.e., sitting down, staying quiet, and looking at the
teacher) prior to the start of all sessions. The visual support remained present and
was used to redirect challenging behavior if it occurred, at the end of each 20-s
interval (i.e., the researcher pointed to the picture that represented the desired
behavior instead of delivering a token) throughout each session.
Token Economy without Perseverative Interest
The token economy system that did not include Troy’s perseverative interest used
pennies with a small patch of Velcro�
on the back that could be fastened to a token
board. Penny tokens were delivered by the researcher sitting near Troy, contingent
on 20-s of consecutive on-task behavior. A maximum of 30 tokens per 10 min
session could be earned. Backup reinforcers (i.e., bite-sized candy) could be
obtained for every 10 tokens earned, and an opportunity to exchange was presented
within sessions at each moment in which Troy had earned 10 tokens. For data
collection purposes, the exchanges were coded as on-task behavior. The token board
included circles drawn in groups of 10 as a visual representation of the number of
tokens needed to earn a backup reinforcer. Upon earning a token for targeted on-
task behaviors, Troy was handed a token to place on the board (also coded as on-
task).
Token Economy with Perseverative Interests
The token economy system used in this condition differed from the previously
described condition in that the pennies and token board were replaced by tokens and
a board related to Troy’s perseverative interest in jigsaw puzzles. Specifically, the
tokens were small foam puzzle pieces, and the token board was a thin cardstock
frame into which the pieces fit. This token board mirrored the traditional token
economy, in that it included 10 outlined locations for each puzzle piece. The same
procedures, response requirements, exchange rate, and backup reinforcers were used
in both token economy conditions (i.e., with and without perseverative interests).
Troy’s perseverative interest in puzzles was determined by interviews with teachers
and a free operant preference assessment, in which a puzzle was made available
alongside other toys and activity options (Roane et al. 1998). All of Troy’s teachers
agreed that he perseverated on a specific puzzle, and he devoted 100 % of his time
in the free operant preference assessment touching, holding, and manipulating the
372 J Behav Educ (2014) 23:368–377
123
puzzle pieces. Further, Troy always selected this specific puzzle when other puzzles
were available.
Generalization
Troy’s behavior during the group reading activity in the inclusion classroom was
measured in one baseline session using the same procedures as the other four
baseline sessions conducted in the life skills classroom. In the final best-treatment
phase of the study, three sessions were conducted in the inclusion classroom using
the perseverative interest token economy system.
Results
The top panel of Fig. 1 displays the percentage of intervals during which Troy
engaged in on-task behavior during the entire 10-s interval. During baseline, Troy
was on-task in the life skills classroom for a mean of 11 % of the intervals (range
8–18 %). In the inclusion classroom, he was on-task during 13 % of the intervals.
During the alternating treatment phase, both token economy interventions resulted
in an increase in on-task behavior relative to baseline. However, Troy was on-task
more often during the perseverative interest token economy condition
(M = 59.7 %, range 48–70 %) than in the token economy condition that did not
involve tokens reflecting his perseverative interest (M = 45 %, range 32–55 %).
The increase in on-task behavior in the perseverative interest condition was then
replicated during the final best-treatment phase in the inclusion classroom
(M = 64 %, range 52–72 %).
The bottom panel of Fig. 1 displays the percentage of 10-s interval during which
at least one instance of challenging behavior occurred. During baseline conditions in
the life skills classroom, challenging behavior occurred during a mean of 89 % of
intervals (range 82–92 %) and in 87 % of intervals in the inclusion classroom.
Challenging behavior decreased from baseline levels in both token economy
conditions; however, a lower percentage of intervals had challenging behavior in the
perseverative interest condition (M = 40 %, range 30–52 %) compared with the
condition where the token did not coincide with Troy’s perseverative interests
(M = 55 %, range 45–68 %). The reduction in challenging behavior in the
perseverative interest condition in the life skills classroom was replicated during the
final best-treatment phase in the inclusion classroom (M = 36 %, range 28–48 %).
Discussion
The results of this study replicate previous research demonstrating the utility of
token economy interventions for children with autism (Matson and Boisjoli 2009)
because both token economy interventions (i.e., with and without the perseverative
interest) resulted in decreased challenging behavior and increased on-task behavior.
Further, the superiority of the condition involving tokens reflecting Troy’s
J Behav Educ (2014) 23:368–377 373
123
perseverative interest is consistent with the findings of Charlop-Christy and Haymes
(1998). Finally, these data extend previous research by demonstrating the benefit of
interest-based tokens in a special education classroom with generalization to an
inclusion classroom.
The perseverative interests inherent to an autism spectrum disorder (ASD)
diagnosis often impede appropriate classroom behavior and learning (e.g., Rispoli
et al. 2011; Lang et al. 2010) and can be associated with serious challenging
behavior (e.g., Hausman et al. 2009; Matson et al. 2009). Thus, interventions have
primarily sought to address challenging behavior associated with such restricted and
repetitive behaviors and interests (RRBI) with antecedent manipulations to enrich
0
10
20
30
40
50
60
70
80
90
100
Per
cent
age
of 1
0-s
Who
le I
nter
vals
w
ith
On-
Tas
k B
ehav
ior
Token Economy Comparison in Baseline PI Token Economy
in Inclusion
Life SkillsClassroom
With PI
Without
0
10
20
30
40
50
60
70
80
90
100
1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18
Per
cent
age
of 1
0-s
Par
tial
Int
erva
lsw
ith
Cha
llen
ging
Beh
avio
r Inclusion Classroom
Probe
Sessions
Fig. 1 The top panel displays the percentage of 10-s whole interval during which Troy was on-task, andthe bottom panel displays the percentage of 10-s partial interval during which Troy engaged inchallenging behavior. The closed circles represent baseline in the life skills classroom, triangles representthe inclusion classroom, open diamonds represent the token economy without the perseverative interest(PI), and closed squares represent the token economy with the PI
374 J Behav Educ (2014) 23:368–377
123
the environment and prevent challenging behaviors, and consequence-based
interventions that involve interrupting the repetitive behavior (see Boyd et al.
2012 for a recent review). However, other researchers have demonstrated the utility
of capitalizing on perseverate interests by incorporating them into the intervention
procedures or making access to RRBI contingent on targeted appropriate behavior
or the absence of target challenging behavior (Baker et al. 1998; Charlop-Christy
and Haymes 1996, 1998; Vismara and Lyons 2011). This study, considered in
tandem with Charlop-Christy and Haymes (1998), suggests idiosyncratic persev-
erative interests can be utilized to improve intervention efficiency and effectiveness.
The putative mechanism of action responsible for the enhanced effectiveness is
likely the increased reinforcing value of the token itself. Compared with the use of
neutral stimuli as tokens, the reinforcement from perseverative interest-based tokens
may be more immediate, and thus more efficient, than relying only on the
reinforcing power of the backup edibles that were available only after a number of
tokens had been earned and exchanged. Although Troy was always willing to
exchange 10 tokens for the backup reinforcers, it is possible that some children may
value the perseverative interest-based tokens more than backup reinforcers. In such
cases, challenging behavior maintained by continued access to preferred tangibles
might be occasioned when the child is asked to exchange the high preferred token
for a less preferred item. Practitioners using this approach are therefore cautioned to
consider the reinforcing value of the perseverative interest token relative to the
backup reinforcers. If challenging behavior is observed during the exchange, it may
be preferable to use neutral stimuli as tokens or to merely use the preservative
interest tokens alone without additional backup reinforcers. As part of a larger effort
to better incorporate the characteristics of children with autism into intervention
approaches with the goal of improving educational outcomes, future research
designed to elucidate and then potentially address such a limitation remains
warranted.
These findings buttress the evidence supporting the use of token economy
systems with this population and align with the perspective that circumscribed
interests can be a unique strength of individuals with high-functioning ASD
(Mercier et al. 2000). Nevertheless, when children with ASD perseverate to the
exclusion of other activities, such RRBIs significantly restrict their social and
learning opportunities (Pierce and Courchesne 2001; Koegel et al. 1974; Lovaas
et al. 1971). Research could continue to investigate the effects of embedding
perseverative interests into other interventions, such as video modeling. However, it
is possible that the use of perseverative interests in this way may inadvertently lead
to a counterproductive increase in fascination with the perseverative interest.
Although we are not aware of this issue having been reported in previous research, it
would seem a plausible potential limitation that should be investigated as research
in this area continues.
The results of this current study should be considered in light of a few limitations.
First, we selected an alternating treatment design because teachers expressed
concern regarding a reversal to baseline conditions. Although this design facilitated
implementation in an applied setting, the lack of a reversal phase introduced the
potential of carryover effects. Second, to identify Troy’s perseverative interest, we
J Behav Educ (2014) 23:368–377 375
123
utilized teacher reports and a free operant preference assessment, which did not
capture a hierarchy of reinforcers (Roane et al. 1998). Further, there is not a well-
established procedure for distinguishing between high preferred stimuli and the
level of fascination indicative of a true perseverative interest, and our assertion that
puzzles were indeed a perseverative interest should be considered with caution. It is
possible that puzzles were merely highly preferred. Future research should further
investigate reinforcement hierarchies to determine more precise ways of identifying
perseverative interests. Third, the visual cues utilized to prompt on-task behavior
and redirect challenging behavior, although held constant in all intervention and
generalization sessions, were not evaluated as a separate intervention component.
Thus, we are uncertain to what degree they may have contributed to the effects on
the dependent variables. Finally, because on-task behavior increased and challeng-
ing behavior decreased with the use of both token systems, assessing the value of
the perseverative interest token proved difficult. It is possible that the effectiveness
of both systems might approach equivalence over time if challenging behavior
continued to decrease. Thus, future research should investigate the effects of
extended use of the two systems, as well as the effects of systematic fading
procedures of the embedded token system.
References
Baker, M. J., Koegel, R. L., & Koegel, L. K. (1998). Increasing the social behavior of young children with
autism using their obsessive behaviors. The Journal of the Association for Persons with Severe
Handicaps, 23, 300–308. doi:10.2511/rpsd.23.4.300.
Boyd, B. A., McDonough, S. G., & Bodfish, J. W. (2012). Evidence-based behavioral interventions for
repetitive behaviors in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 42, 1236–1248.
doi:10.1007/s10803-011-1284-z.
Charlop-Christy, M. H., & Haymes, L. K. (1996). Using obsessions as reinforcers with and without mild
reductive procedures to decrease inappropriate behaviors of children with autism. Journal of Autism
and Developmental Disorders, 26, 527–545.
Charlop-Christy, M. H., & Haymes, L. K. (1998). Using objects of obsession as token reinforcers for
children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 28, 189–198. doi:10.1023/A:
1026061220171.
Fisher, W. W., Piazza, C. C., Bowman, L. G., Hagopian, L. P., Owens, J. C., & Slevin, I. (1992). A
comparison of two approaches for identifying reinforcers for persons with severe and profound
disabilities. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 25, 491–498. doi:10.1901/jaba.1992.25-491.
Gast, D. L. (2010). Single subject research methodology in behavioral sciences. New York: Routledge.
Hackenberg, T. D. (2009). Token reinforcement: A review and analysis. Journal of the Experimental
Analysis of Behavior, 91, 257–286. doi:10.1901/jeab.2009.91-257.
Hausman, N., Kahng, S., Farrell, E., & Mongeon, C. (2009). Idiosyncratic functions: Severe problem
behavior maintained by access to ritualistic behaviors. Education and Treatment of Children, 32,
77–87.
Kennedy, C. (2005). Single-case designs for educational research. Boston: Pearson Education Inc.
Koegel, R. L., Firestone, P. B., Kramme, K. W., & Dunlap, G. (1974). Increasing spontaneous play by
suppressing self-stimulation in autistic children. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 7, 521–528.
Lang, R., Regester, A., Rispoli, M., & Camargo, S. H. (2010). Rehabilitation issues for children with
autism spectrum disorders. Developmental Neurorehabilitation, 13, 153–155.
Lovaas, O. I., Schreibman, L., Koegel, R., & Rehm, R. (1971). Selective responding by autistic children
to multiple sensory input. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 77(3), 211–222.
376 J Behav Educ (2014) 23:368–377
123
Maggin, D. M., Chafouleas, S. M., Goddard, K. M., & Johnson, A. H. (2011). A systematic evaluation of
token economies as a classroom management tool for students with challenging behavior. Journal of
School Psychology, 49, 529–554. doi:10.1016/j.jsp.2011.05.001.
Matson, J. L., & Boisjoli, J. A. (2009). The token economy for children with intellectual disability and/or
autism: A review. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 30, 240–248. doi:10.1016/j.ridd.2008.04.
001.
Matson, J. L., Dempsey, T., & Fodstad, J. C. (2009). Stereotypies and repetitive/restrictive behaviours in
infants with autism and pervasive developmental disorder. Developmental Neurorehabilitation, 12,
122–127.
Matson, J. L., Tureck, K., & Rieske, R. (2012). The questions about behavioral function (QABF): Current
status as a method of functional assessment. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 33, 630–634.
doi:10.1016/j.ridd.2011.11.006.
Mercier, C., Mottron, L., & Belleville, S. (2000). A psychosocial study on restricted interests in high-
functioning persons with pervasive developmental disorders. Autism, 4(4), 29–46.
Mottram, L., & Berger-Gross, P. (2004). An intervention to reduce disruptive behaviours in children with
brain injury. Developmental Neurorehabilitation, 7, 133–143.
Pierce, K., & Courchesne, E. (2001). Evidence for a cerebellar role in reduced exploration and
stereotyped behavior in autism. Biological Psychiatry, 49, 655–664.
Reynolds, C. R., & Kamphaus, R. W. (2004). BASC-II: Behavior assessment system for children (2nd
ed.). Bloomington, MN: Pearson Assessments.
Rispoli, M. J., O’Reilly, M. F., Lang, R., Machalicek, W., Davis, T., Lancioni, G., et al. (2011). Effects of
motivating operations on aberrant behavior and academic engagement for two students with autism.
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 44, 187–192.
Roane, H. S., Vollmer, T. R., Ringdahl, J. E., & Marcus, B. A. (1998). Evaluation of a brief stimulus
preference assessment. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 31, 605–620. doi:10.1901/jaba.1998.
31-605.
Schopler, E., Reichler, R. J., Devellis, R. F., & Daly, K. (1980). Toward an objective classification of
childhood autism: Childhood autism rating scale (CARS). Journal of Autism and Developmental
Disabilities, 10, 91–103. doi:10.1007/BF02408436.
Vismara, L. A., & Lyons, G. L. (2011). Using perseverative interest to elicit joint attention behaviors in
young children with autism: Theoretical and clinical implications for understanding motivation.
Journal of Positive Behavioral Interventions, 9(4), 214–228. doi:10.1177/10983007070090040401.
J Behav Educ (2014) 23:368–377 377
123
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited withoutpermission.
- c.10864_2014_Article_9195.pdf
- Effects of a Perseverative Interest-Based Token Economy on Challenging and On-Task Behavior in a Child with Autism
- Abstract
- Introduction
- Method
- Participant, Setting, and Materials
- Response Measurement and Interobserver Agreement
- Procedure
- Research Design
- Baseline
- Preference and Backup Reinforcers
- Token Economy without Perseverative Interest
- Token Economy with Perseverative Interests
- Generalization
- Results
- Discussion
- References